The costs and benefits of cross-task priming

Two lines of research on cross-task priming yield opposite results. Research on repetition priming observed positive priming, whereas research on the role of priming in task-switching observed negative effects. We combined the two types of design. In the transfer phase of our paradigm, subjects performed task B either as a pure block (BBB) or as a switch block (ABAB). We presented items which were either unprimed or primed by prior presentation during a preceding priming phase performed on task A. Amongst others, the priming effect is determined by two factors: First, the more operation time the system needs during the probe event, the higher the likelihood to obtain priming. Protracting operation time by reducing stimulus quality favors positive priming, whereas providing more operation time by making subjects switch between tasks favors negative priming. Second, the strength of the memory trace of the prime event determines whether that trace can possibly yield negative priming, in that only strong traces can be retrieved together with the associated task/response.

[1]  Delos D. Wickens,et al.  Encoding Categories of Words: An Empirical Approach to Meaning. , 1970 .

[2]  C. A. Becker Allocation of attention during visual word recognition. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Thomas H. Killion,et al.  Interaction of visual and cognitive effects in word recognition. , 1977 .

[4]  Don L. Scarborough,et al.  Accessing lexical memory: The transfer of word repetition effects across task and modality , 1979 .

[5]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[6]  Dennis Norris,et al.  The Effects of Frequency, Repetition and Stimulus Quality in Visual Word Recognition , 1984 .

[7]  A. Ellis Progress in the psychology of language , 1985 .

[8]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[9]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[10]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Measures of Memory , 1988 .

[11]  A. Young,et al.  Repetition Priming and Face Processing: Priming Occurs within the System that Responds to the Identity of a Face , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[12]  G. Logan Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  S P Tipper,et al.  Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: time course and disruption. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  G D Logan,et al.  The loss of repetition priming and automaticity over time as a function of degree of initial learning , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[15]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[16]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  What Is Learned During Automatization? The Role of Attention in Constructing an Instance , 1994 .

[17]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[18]  A. Treisman,et al.  Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[20]  W. Neill Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. , 1997 .

[21]  W. Singer,et al.  Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time-lag synchronization among cortical areas , 1997, Nature.

[22]  D. Lowe Long-term positive and negative identity priming: Evidence for episodic retrieval , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[23]  B. Hommel Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .

[24]  S. Wise,et al.  Role of the Hippocampal System in Conditional Motor Learning: Mapping Antecedents to Action , 1999, Hippocampus.

[25]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[27]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Contextualization in perception and action , 2000 .

[28]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) , 2000 .

[29]  T. McNamara,et al.  Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[30]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  I. Koch The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  R. Henson Neuroimaging studies of priming , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[36]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  B. Hommel,et al.  Moderate alcohol consumption in humans impairs feature binding in visual perception but not across perception and action , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  B. Hommel,et al.  Semantic generalization of stimulus-task bindings , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  G. Gilmore,et al.  Stimulus Contrast and Word Reading Speed in Alzheimer's Disease , 2005, Experimental aging research.

[41]  B. Hommel,et al.  Caffeine, but not nicotine, enhances visual feature binding , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Interaction of task readiness and automatic retrieval in task switching: Negative priming and competitor priming , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[43]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Involuntary retrieval in alphabet-arithmetic tasks: Task-mixing and task-switching costs , 2005, Psychological research.

[44]  A. Allport,et al.  Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[45]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .