Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space

There is now considerable evidence that space is compressed when stimuli are flashed shortly before or after the onset of a saccadic eye movement. Here we report that short intervals of time between two successive perisaccadic visual (but not auditory) stimuli are also underestimated, indicating a compression of perceived time. We were even more surprised that in a critical interval before saccades, perceived temporal order is consistently reversed. The very similar time courses of spatial and temporal compression suggest that both are mediated by a common neural mechanism, probably related to the predictive shifts that occur in receptive fields of many visual areas at the time of saccades.

[1]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[2]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[3]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Dynamic allocation of visual attention during the execution of sequences of saccades , 2004, Vision Research.

[6]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[7]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal Impulse Response Functions for Luminance and Colour During Saccades , 1996, Vision Research.

[8]  J T Enns,et al.  The duration of a brief event in the mind's eye. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.

[9]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  P. Tse,et al.  Attention and the subjective expansion of time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  Global Transsaccadic Change Blindness During Scene Perception , 2003, Psychological science.

[12]  John C. Rothwell,et al.  Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity , 2001, Nature.

[13]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[14]  E. Titchener Scientific Books: Lectures on the Elementary Psychology of Feeling and Attention , 1909 .

[15]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.

[17]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[18]  G. McConkie,et al.  Is visual information integrated across successive fixations in reading? , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  G. Sperling,et al.  Attention gating in short-term visual memory. , 1986, Psychological review.

[20]  R. Church,et al.  Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.

[21]  C. Spence,et al.  Visual Prior Entry , 2001, Psychological science.

[22]  S. B. Stevenson,et al.  Dependence of visual suppression on the amplitudes of saccades and blinks , 1986, Vision Research.

[23]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.

[24]  D Rose,et al.  Duration Illusions in a Train of Visual Stimuli , 1995, Perception.

[25]  W. Ridder,et al.  Suppression of contrast sensitivity during eyelid blinks , 1993, Vision Research.

[26]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. 7/4, Visual Psychophysics , 1973 .

[27]  Marcus Kaiser,et al.  Perisaccadic Mislocalization Orthogonal to Saccade Direction , 2004, Neuron.

[28]  Kielan Yarrow,et al.  Manual Chronostasis Tactile Perception Precedes Physical Contact , 2003, Current Biology.

[29]  Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al.  Spatial localization precedes temporal determination in visual perception , 2003, Vision Research.

[30]  B. Libet,et al.  Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: a functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  B. Libet,et al.  Subjective Referral of the Timing for a Conscious Sensory Experience , 1979 .

[32]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Auditory Chronostasis Hanging on the Telephone , 2002, Current Biology.

[34]  Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al.  Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[36]  D. Burr,et al.  Saccadic compression can improve detection of Glass patterns , 2002, Vision Research.

[37]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[38]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[39]  E. Titchener The Psychology of Feeling and Attention , 2022 .

[40]  G. Sperling,et al.  Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial attention. , 1995 .

[41]  M. Sanders Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .

[42]  P. Haggard,et al.  Voluntary action and conscious awareness , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.