Continuous Environmental Changes May Enhance Topographic Memory Skills. Evidence From L’Aquila Earthquake-Exposed Survivors

Exposure to environmental contextual changes, such as those occurring after an earthquake, requires individuals to learn novel routes around their environment, landmarks and spatial layout. In this study, we aimed to uncover whether contextual changes that occurred after the 2009 L’Aquila earthquake affected topographic memory in exposed survivors. We hypothesized that individuals exposed to environmental changes—individuals living in L’Aquila before, during and after the earthquake (hereafter called exposed participants, EPs)—improved their topographic memory skills compared with non-exposed participants (NEPs) who moved to L’Aquila after the earthquake, as only EPs had to modify their previous cognitive map of L’Aquila. We also hypothesized that memory improvement was selective for the navigational space and did not generalize across other spatial and verbal domains. To test these hypotheses, we compared the topographic and spatial memory skills of 56 EPs without post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms to the skills of 47 NEPs using the Walking Corsi Test (WalCT; memory test in the navigational space) and the Corsi Block-Tapping Test (CBT; visuospatial memory test in the reaching space); EPs and NEPs were matched for gender, education and general navigational skills. A sub-group of participants also underwent the Rey-Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; verbal memory test). The results showed that only EPs had better performances on topographic learning (TL) assessed using the WalCT rather than spatial learning assessed by the CBT. This outcome suggests the possibility that EPs specifically improved topographic memory. This effect may be due to continuous exposure to environmental changes that have required individuals to learn novel paths within the city and integrate novel information, such as “new towns,” into their pre-existing mental representation of the city. Implications and limitations of the study are discussed.

[1]  Alessandro O. Caffò,et al.  Topographical disorientation in aging. Familiarity with the environment does matter , 2018, Neurological Sciences.

[2]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Enhancing Allocentric Spatial Recall in Pre-schoolers through Navigational Training Programme , 2017, Front. Neurosci..

[3]  M. Palmiero,et al.  The Role of Emotional Landmarks on Topographical Memory , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[4]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Does the Cerebellum Contribute to Human Navigation by Processing Sequential Information? , 2017, Neuropsychology.

[5]  A. Giannini,et al.  Personality Traits and Coping Strategies for Contrasting the Occurrence of Traumatic Reactions in Emergency Rescuers , 2016 .

[6]  Ghassan El-Baalbaki,et al.  A Multidimensional Approach to Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder - from Theory to Practice , 2016 .

[7]  Fabio Ferlazzo,et al.  Domain-Specific Interference Tests on Navigational Working Memory in Military Pilots. , 2016, Aerospace medicine and human performance.

[8]  M. Palmiero,et al.  Sex differences in visuospatial and navigational working memory: the role of mood induced by background music , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  L. Piccardi,et al.  [Differences in coping strategies of preadolescents with and without exposure to the L'Aquila (Central Italy) 2009 earthquake]. , 2016, Epidemiologia e prevenzione.

[10]  J. Schüler,et al.  Gymnasts and orienteers display better mental rotation performance than nonathletes , 2016 .

[11]  A. Giannini,et al.  Different neural modifications underpin PTSD after different traumatic events: an fMRI meta-analytic study , 2016, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[12]  F. Ferlazzo,et al.  A dedicated system for topographical working memory: evidence from domain-specific interference tests , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Paola Verde,et al.  Gender differences in navigational memory: pilots vs. nonpilots. , 2015, Aerospace medicine and human performance.

[14]  Friedrich Barnikel,et al.  Teaching Spatial Competence Today — From Analogue Maps to Geocaching , 2014, KN - Journal of Cartography and Geographic Information.

[15]  Thomas Wolbers,et al.  Challenges for identifying the neural mechanisms that support spatial navigation: the impact of spatial scale , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  C. Guariglia,et al.  A Selective Egocentric Topographical Working Memory Deficit in the Early Stages of Alzheimer’s Disease , 2014, American journal of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias.

[17]  Thomas Blaschke,et al.  Myths and realities about the recovery of L׳Aquila after the earthquake , 2014, International journal of disaster risk reduction : IJDRR.

[18]  Laura Piccardi,et al.  Development of navigational working memory: evidence from 6- to 10-year-old children. , 2014, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[19]  Federico Nemmi,et al.  Looking for the compass in a case of developmental topographical disorientation: A behavioral and neuroimaging study , 2014, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[20]  Federico Nemmi,et al.  Where Am I? A new case of developmental topographical disorientation. , 2014, Journal of neuropsychology.

[21]  M. Preisig,et al.  Determinants of the development of post-traumatic stress disorder, in the general population , 2014, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.

[22]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Neuropsychology of Environmental Navigation in Humans: Review and Meta-Analysis of fMRI Studies in Healthy Participants , 2014, Neuropsychology Review.

[23]  C. Guariglia,et al.  The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): A Normative Study of Topographical Working Memory in a Sample of 4- to 11-Year-Olds , 2014, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[24]  A. Marano,et al.  Perceived Self-Efficacy and Coping Styles Related to Stressful Critical Life Events , 2013, PloS one.

[25]  Federico Nemmi,et al.  Segregation of neural circuits involved in spatial learning in reaching and navigational space , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  C. Guariglia,et al.  The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): standardization of the topographical memory test in an Italian population , 2013, Neurological Sciences.

[27]  Carol S. Fullerton,et al.  A Systematic Review of PTSD Prevalence and Trajectories in DSM-5 Defined Trauma Exposed Populations: Intentional and Non-Intentional Traumatic Events , 2013, PloS one.

[28]  David Alexander,et al.  An evaluation of medium-term recovery processes after the 6 April 2009 earthquake in L'Aquila, Central Italy , 2013 .

[29]  A. Elklit,et al.  Gender, trauma type, and PTSD prevalence: a re-analysis of 18 nordic convenience samples , 2012, Annals of General Psychiatry.

[30]  G. Iaria,et al.  A specific deficit in spatial memory acquisition in post‐traumatic stress disorder and the role of sleep in its consolidation , 2012, Hippocampus.

[31]  Raffaella Nori,et al.  Il senso dell'orientamento: Quanto conta la familiarità con l'ambiente? , 2012 .

[32]  Russell A. Epstein,et al.  Neural responses to visual scenes reveals inconsistencies between fMRI adaptation and multivoxel pattern analysis , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  L. Piccardi,et al.  Familiarity and Environmental Representations of a City: A Self-Report Study , 2011, Psychological reports.

[34]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Dissociated deficits of visuo-spatial memory in near space and navigational space: Evidence from brain-damaged patients and healthy older participants , 2011, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[35]  R. Nori,et al.  The trauma symptom inventory: Italian validation of an instrument for the assessment of post-traumatic symptoms , 2011, Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences.

[36]  D. Alexander The L'Aquila Earthquake of 6 April 2009 and Italian Government Policy on Disaster Response , 2010 .

[37]  B. McEwen,et al.  Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[38]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Different spatial memory systems are involved in small- and large-scale environments: evidence from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Developmental topographical disorientation in a healthy subject , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  G. Bonanno,et al.  Weighing the Costs of Disaster , 2010, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[41]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Developmental topographical disorientation: Case one , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Pure representational neglect and navigational deficits in a case with preserved visuo-spatial working memory , 2008, Neurocase.

[43]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Navigational skills correlate with hippocampal fractional anisotropy in humans , 2008, Hippocampus.

[44]  G. Iaria,et al.  Walking in the Corsi test: Which type of memory do you need? , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[45]  J. Pratt,et al.  Playing an Action Video Game Reduces Gender Differences in Spatial Cognition , 2007, Psychological science.

[46]  M. Petrides,et al.  Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  G. Iaria,et al.  Lack of orientation due to a congenital brain malformation: A case study , 2005, Neurocase.

[48]  Mazlan Mahadzir,et al.  Improving spatial ability using a Web-based Virtual Environment (WbVE) , 2005 .

[49]  S. Galea,et al.  Disparities in mental health treatment following the World Trade Center Disaster: implications for mental health care and health services research. , 2005, Journal of traumatic stress.

[50]  Tomaso Vecchi,et al.  Aging and everyday memory: the beneficial effect of memory training. , 2003, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[51]  R. de Lisi,et al.  Improving Children's Mental Rotation Accuracy With Computer Game Playing , 2002, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[52]  C. Whitfield Psychoneuroimmunology: Stress, Mental Disorders and Health. , 2002 .

[53]  M. Elliott,et al.  A national survey of stress reactions after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. , 2001, The New England journal of medicine.

[54]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Orientation Working Memory , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  E. D. Haan,et al.  Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[56]  H. Heinze,et al.  Cortical Activations during the Mental Rotation of Different Visual Objects , 2001, NeuroImage.

[57]  R. Sapolsky,et al.  How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. , 2000, Endocrine reviews.

[58]  S. Southwick,et al.  Neural correlates of memories of childhood sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[59]  Melissa Dugger,et al.  Eliminating Gender Differences Through Practice in an Applied Visual Spatial Task , 1998 .

[60]  A Berthoz,et al.  Parietal and hippocampal contribution to topokinetic and topographic memory. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[61]  D. Cicchetti Emotion and Adaptation , 1993 .

[62]  G. Edelman Neural Darwinism: The Theory Of Neuronal Group Selection , 1989 .

[63]  S. Hobfoll Conservation of resources. A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[64]  M. Pépin,et al.  Effect of Playing a Video Game on a Measure of Spatial Visualization , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[65]  E. Schachtel On memory and childhood amnesia. , 1947, Psychiatry.

[66]  M. Palmiero,et al.  Neuro-functional alterations due to PTSD after environmental disasters: fMRI evidence and clinical suggestions , 2016 .

[67]  D. A. Arcarazo,et al.  Myths and Realities , 2015 .

[68]  J. Sareen,et al.  The association between number and type of traumatic life experiences and physical conditions in a nationally representative sample. , 2014, General hospital psychiatry.

[69]  Friedrich Barnikel,et al.  “Geocaching“ as a Method to Improve not only Spatial but also Social Skills: Results from a School Project , 2014 .

[70]  Raffaella Nori,et al.  FAMILIARITY AND SPATIAL COGNITIVE STYLE: HOW IMPORTANT ARE THEY FOR SPATIAL REPRESENTATION? , 2011 .

[71]  J. Bremner Hypotheses and controversies related to effects of stress on the hippocampus: An argument for stress‐induced damage to the hippocampus in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder , 2001, Hippocampus.

[72]  C. Holahan,et al.  Long-term posttreatment functioning among patients with unipolar depression: an integrative model. , 2000, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[73]  A. Morral,et al.  Influence of emotional engagement and habituation on exposure therapy for PTSD. , 1998, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[74]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  The Mental Deterioration Battery: normative data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. The Group for the Standardization of the Mental Deterioration Battery. , 1996, European neurology.

[75]  D. Meichenbaum,et al.  Stress inoculation: a preventative approach. , 1985, Issues in mental health nursing.

[76]  Philip M. Corsi Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. , 1972 .

[77]  H. Teuber,et al.  Spatial Orientation in Man After Cerebral Injury: I. Analyses by Locus of Lesion , 1955 .

[78]  N. Alpert,et al.  See Blockindiscussions, Blockinstats, Blockinand Blockinauthor Blockinprofiles Blockinfor Blockinthis Blockinpublication Hippocampal Blockinfunction Blockinin Blockinposttraumatic Blockinstress Disorder Hippocampal Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder the Goal of the Current Research Was to Use , 2022 .