Media Multitasking and Cognitive, Psychological, Neural, and Learning Differences

American youth spend more time with media than any other waking activity: an average of 7.5 hours per day, every day. On average, 29% of that time is spent juggling multiple media streams simultaneously (ie, media multitasking). This phenomenon is not limited to American youth but is paralleled across the globe. Given that a large number of media multitaskers (MMTs) are children and young adults whose brains are still developing, there is great urgency to understand the neurocognitive profiles of MMTs. It is critical to understand the relation between the relevant cognitive domains and underlying neural structure and function. Of equal importance is understanding the types of information processing that are necessary in 21st century learning environments. The present review surveys the growing body of evidence demonstrating that heavy MMTs show differences in cognition (eg, poorer memory), psychosocial behavior (eg, increased impulsivity), and neural structure (eg, reduced volume in anterior cingulate cortex). Furthermore, research indicates that multitasking with media during learning (in class or at home) can negatively affect academic outcomes. Until the direction of causality is understood (whether media multitasking causes such behavioral and neural differences or whether individuals with such differences tend to multitask with media more often), the data suggest that engagement with concurrent media streams should be thoughtfully considered. Findings from such research promise to inform policy and practice on an increasingly urgent societal issue while significantly advancing our understanding of the intersections between cognitive, psychosocial, neural, and academic factors.

[1]  Larry D. Rosen,et al.  Causes, effects, and practicalities of everyday multitasking , 2015 .

[2]  Matthew S Cain,et al.  Media multitasking in adolescence , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Susanne E. Baumgartner,et al.  Measuring Media Multitasking: Development of a Short Measure of Media Multitasking for Adolescents , 2017, J. Media Psychol. Theor. Methods Appl..

[5]  Johannes W. J. Beentjes,et al.  Background Television as an Inhibitor of Performance on Easy and Difficult Homework Assignments , 2000, Commun. Res..

[6]  Anastasia Kononova,et al.  Multitasking Across Borders: A Cross-National Study of Media Multitasking Behaviors, Its Antecedents, and Outcomes , 2013 .

[7]  V. Rideout,et al.  Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8- to 18-Year-Olds , 2010 .

[8]  Meredith Minear,et al.  Working memory, fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media multitaskers , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[9]  Wade C. Jacobsen,et al.  The Wired Generation: Academic and Social Outcomes of Electronic Media Use Among University Students , 2011, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[10]  M. Moisala,et al.  Media multitasking is associated with distractibility and increased prefrontal activity in adolescents and young adults , 2016, NeuroImage.

[11]  C Shawn Green,et al.  Short-term mindfulness intervention reduces the negative attentional effects associated with heavy media multitasking , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[12]  R. Pea,et al.  Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[13]  C. S. Green,et al.  Technology consumption and cognitive control: Contrasting action video game experience with media multitasking , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Clifford Nass,et al.  Cognitive control in media multitaskers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  Karin Archer,et al.  Examining the impact of off-task multi-tasking with technology on real-time classroom learning , 2012, Comput. Educ..

[16]  D. Strayer,et al.  Who Multi-Tasks and Why? Multi-Tasking Ability, Perceived Multi-Tasking Ability, Impulsivity, and Sensation Seeking , 2013, PloS one.

[17]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Media multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained attention , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[18]  Raquel Benbunan-Fich,et al.  Self-interruptions in discretionary multitasking , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[19]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Higher Media Multi-Tasking Activity Is Associated with Smaller Gray-Matter Density in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2014, PloS one.

[20]  Shelia R. Cotten,et al.  Perceived academic effects of instant messaging use , 2011, Comput. Educ..

[21]  Michael S. Gendron,et al.  Can students really multitask? An experimental study of instant messaging while reading , 2010, Comput. Educ..

[22]  Larry D. Rosen,et al.  Facebook and texting made me do it: Media-induced task-switching while studying , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[23]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  The effects of background television on the toy play behavior of very young children. , 2008, Child development.

[24]  Tip Robertson,et al.  Reading Performances Between Novices and Experts in Different Media Multitasking Environments , 2009 .

[25]  Patti M. Valkenburg,et al.  The consequences of media multitasking for youth: A review , 2015, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[26]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  Media Multitasking Is Associated with Symptoms of Depression and Social Anxiety , 2013, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[27]  Anthony D. Wagner,et al.  Media multitasking and memory: Differences in working memory and long-term memory , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Play = Learning: How Play Motivates and Enhances Children's Cognitive and Social-Emotional Growth , 2006 .

[29]  S. Shih A Null Relationship between Media Multitasking and Well-Being , 2013, PloS one.