Evidence for motor simulation in imagined locomotion.

A series of experiments examined the role of the motor system in imagined movement, finding a strong relationship between imagined walking performance and the biomechanical information available during actual walking. Experiments 1 through 4 established the finding that real and imagined locomotion differ in absolute walking time. We then tested whether executed actions could provide a basis for imagined walking rate using 2 approaches. Experiments 5 and 6 used a perceptual-motor recalibration paradigm, finding that after physically walking in a treadmill virtual reality environment, actors recalibrated the time to imagine walking to a previously viewed target. This finding mirrors previous perceptual-motor recalibration work measuring actual walking to previously viewed targets. Experiments 7 and 8 used a dual-task paradigm in which actions performed concurrently with imagined walking increased the similarity between real and imagined walking time, but only when they were biomechanically consistent with the act of walking. The striking influence of biomechanical information on imagined locomotion provides evidence for shared motor systems in imagined and executed movements and is also directly relevant to the mechanisms involved in egocentric spatial updating of environmental layout.

[1]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  J. Decety,et al.  Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[6]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort , 1991, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Central activation of autonomic effectors during mental simulation of motor actions in man. , 1993, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  J Decety,et al.  Analysis of actual and mental movement times in graphic tasks. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[10]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[13]  M. Jeannerod Mental imagery in the motor context , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[18]  N. Stucchi,et al.  Viewer- and object-centered mental explorations of an imagined environment are not equivalent. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[19]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.

[20]  A. Wohlschläger,et al.  Mental and manual rotation. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Automatic Updating of Body-Centered Spatial Relationships , 1998 .

[22]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Motor processes in mental rotation , 1998, Cognition.

[23]  John J. Rieser,et al.  The recalibration of rotational locomotion , 1999 .

[24]  Adar Pelah,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation without vision? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Mary C. Whitton,et al.  Walking > walking-in-place > flying, in virtual environments , 1999, SIGGRAPH.

[26]  K Matsuyama,et al.  Stimulation of a restricted region in the midline cerebellar white matter evokes coordinated quadrupedal locomotion in the decerebrate cat. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Path integration while ignoring irrelevant movement. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  Dennis Velakoulis,et al.  The voluntary control of motor imagery. Imagined movements in individuals with feigned motor impairment and conversion disorder , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[30]  M. de Vega,et al.  Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation , 2001 .

[31]  K Matsuyama,et al.  Supraspinal sites that induce locomotion in the vertebrate central nervous system. , 2001, Advances in neurology.

[32]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[33]  Christoph Stippich,et al.  Somatotopic mapping of the human primary sensorimotor cortex during motor imagery and motor execution by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[34]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Selective Activation of a Parietofrontal Circuit during Implicitly Imagined Prehension , 2002, NeuroImage.

[35]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of direction and mass , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  Redirected Walking in Place , 2002, EGVE.

[37]  Stefan Müller,et al.  Proceedings of the workshop on Virtual environments 2002 , 2002 .

[38]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[40]  M. Hallett,et al.  Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Stefan Geyer,et al.  Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Maryjane Wraga,et al.  Thinking outside the body: an advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.

[44]  R. Klatzky,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Use of Cognitive Versus Perceptual Heading During Imagined Locomotion Depends on the Response Mode , 2022 .

[45]  Martin Wiesmann,et al.  Brain activation patterns during imagined stance and locomotion in functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, NeuroImage.

[46]  M. E. McCarty,et al.  Reaching beyond spatial perception: Effects of intended future actions on visually guided prehension , 2004 .

[47]  James F. Cremer,et al.  Distance perception in real and virtual environments , 2004 .

[48]  G. Courtine,et al.  Gait-dependent motor memory facilitation in covert movement execution. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[49]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Path Integration Deficits during Linear Locomotion after Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  M. May,et al.  Imaginal perspective switches in remembered environments: Transformation versus interference accounts , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[51]  C Collet,et al.  Duration of Mentally Simulated Movement: A Review , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[52]  J. A. Stevens,et al.  Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action , 2005, Cognition.

[53]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[54]  Laura F. Fox,et al.  Self-motion perception during locomotor recalibration: more than meets the eye. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  F. P. de Lange,et al.  Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  The influence of feedback on egocentric distance judgments in real and virtual environments , 2006, APGV '06.

[57]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Lateral somatotopic organization during imagined and prepared movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment , 2007, TAP.

[59]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Visual flow influences gait transition speed and preferred walking speed , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  David Waller,et al.  Correcting distance estimates by interacting with immersive virtual environments: effects of task and available sensory information. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[61]  R. Golledge Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes , 2010 .