Brain Activities Related to the Chinese Character Chunking Tasks: An fMRI Study

Chunking is a unifying information-processing mechanism in human cognition and plays an important role in problem solving. In the present experiment, we use a Chinese character chunking task to explore the neural basis of the goal-oriented chunking, which were classified into loose chunking and tight chunking. The behavioral result confirmed our prediction that the tight chunking was more difficult than the loose chunking. For the imaging result, we observed similar neural activations in the occipital, parietal and frontal lobule in both conditions, which may reflect the visual perception, visuo-spatial and executive processing in the manipulation of Chinese chunks; by direct contrast the tight chunking and loose chunking, we found stronger activation in bilateral cuneus and we suggested that it may be resulted from the more mental operation of visual mental manipulation needed in the tight chunking.

[1]  J. Desmond,et al.  Neural Substrates of Fluid Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Neocortical Activation during Performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Kewei Chen,et al.  Cerebral asymmetry in children when reading Chinese characters. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  Chantal E. Stern,et al.  An fMRI investigation of the role of the basal ganglia in reasoning , 2007, Brain Research.

[4]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Mental imagery and chunks: Empirical and computational findings , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[5]  P. Fox,et al.  The Neural System Underlying Chinese Logograph Reading , 2001, NeuroImage.

[6]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[8]  Ying Liu,et al.  The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.

[9]  Hsuan-Chih Chen,et al.  The Visual Word Form Area: Evidence from an fMRI study of implicit processing of Chinese characters , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  M. Denis,et al.  Neural basis of mental scanning of a topographic representation built from a text. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  L. Tan,et al.  Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture , 2004, Nature.

[12]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Human Brain Activity Related to the Perception of Spatial Features of Objects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[13]  M. Farah,et al.  A functional MRI study of mental image generation , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Steven Phillips,et al.  Neural correlates of the ‘Aha! reaction’ , 2004, Neuroreport.

[15]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  The mind's eye in blindfold chess , 2005 .

[16]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  The Mental Number Line and the Human Angular Gyrus , 2001, NeuroImage.

[18]  Kazuhisa Niki,et al.  Function of hippocampus in “insight” of problem solving , 2003, Hippocampus.

[19]  Ronald A. Finke,et al.  Imagery, Creativity, and Emergent Structure , 1996, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  Kazuhisa Niki,et al.  Perceptual contributions to problem solving: Chunk decomposition of Chinese characters , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[21]  De Groot,et al.  Thought and choice in chess, 2nd ed. , 1978 .

[22]  N. Tzourio,et al.  Different mental imagery abilities result in different regional cerebral blood flow activation patterns during cognitive tasks , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  J. Pine,et al.  Chunking mechanisms in human learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  P. Fox,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta‐analysis , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[25]  L. Tan,et al.  Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  Stellan Ohlsson,et al.  The Mechanism of Restructuring in Geometry , 1990 .

[27]  S. Ohlsson,et al.  Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving , 1999 .

[28]  M. Petrides Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  J. Duncan,et al.  Encoding Strategies Dissociate Prefrontal Activity from Working Memory Demand , 2003, Neuron.

[30]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning , 2001, NeuroImage.

[31]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[32]  Yiping Chen,et al.  Effects of Word Form on Brain Processing of Written Chinese , 2002, NeuroImage.

[33]  Po-Lei Lee,et al.  Orthographic and phonological processing of Chinese characters: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[34]  X. Mai,et al.  “Aha!” effects in a guessing riddle task: An event‐related potential study , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Edward M. Bowden,et al.  Neural Activity When People Solve Verbal Problems with Insight , 2004, PLoS biology.

[36]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.