Interaction of attentional and lexical processes in neglect dyslexia

Abstract Two cases of “left” neglect dyslexia are examined. The two patients show qualitatively similar problems of reading behaviour, although they differ quantitatively in terms of the exposure durations under which neglect dyslexia is manifest. Like other neglect patients documented in the literature, the two patients made mainly substitution errors to the left-hand letter in words, tending to misname the word as a neighbour of similar length. Unlike some patients, the two patients here showed lexical effects (i.e. they performed better with words than with nonwords, and they found words with many orthographic neighbours more difficult to identify than those with few orthographic neighbours). In addition, the patients were susceptible to attempts to manipulate the locus of attention, they were induced into producing addition errors by placing a hash at the left-hand end of letter strings, and their neglect reflected the position of the letter in the string rather than in the visual field. The argument ...

[1]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  Different impairments contribute to neglect dyslexia , 1991 .

[2]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Levels of representation, co-ordinate frames, and unilateral neglect , 1990 .

[3]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  M. Mozer,et al.  Perceptual and Conceptual Mechanisms in Neglect Dyslexia: Two Contrasting Case Studies , 1990 .

[5]  V. Bruce,et al.  Visual Cognition: Computational, Experimental, and Neuropsychological Perspectives , 1989 .

[6]  E. Warrington,et al.  Unilateral visual neglect overcome by cues implicit in stimulus arrays. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[7]  R. Bruyer,et al.  Ipsilateral influences on contralateral processing in neglect patients , 1989 .

[8]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Recognition of visual letter strings following injury to the posterior visual spatial attention system , 1988 .

[9]  A. Young,et al.  “Neglect dyslexia” and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords , 1987 .

[10]  D. Perani,et al.  Unilateral neglect: Personal and extra-personal , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  G Gainotti,et al.  Mechanisms of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  A F Monk,et al.  Theoretical Note: Coordinate Systems in Visual Word Recognition , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  D. LaBerge Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  M. Posner,et al.  Neural systems control of spatial orienting. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[18]  D. Green,et al.  Detecting targets in letter and non-letter arrays. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .

[20]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[21]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[22]  James C Johnston,et al.  A test of the Sophisticated Guessing Theory of word perception , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  E. Warrington,et al.  Word-comprehension and word-retrieval in patients with localized cerebral lesions. , 1978, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Target position and practice in the identification of letters in varying contexts: A word superiority effect , 1976 .

[25]  P. Merikle,et al.  On the selective effects of a patterned masking stimulus. , 1971, Canadian journal of psychology.

[26]  S. Anstis,et al.  Visual delay as a function of luminance. , 1969, The American journal of psychology.

[27]  BELA JULESZ,et al.  Short Term Visual Memory and the Pulfrich Phenomenon , 1969, Nature.

[28]  G. Gainotti Les Manifestations De Negligence et D'inattention Pour L'hemispace , 1968 .

[29]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  A variety of reading disability associated with right hemisphere lesions , 1962 .

[30]  E. Warrington,et al.  A disorder of simultaneous form perception. , 1962, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  A. Luria,et al.  Disorders of "simultaneous perception" in a case of bilateral occipito-parietal brain injury. , 1959, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  X. Seron,et al.  A Retrospective Analysis of a Single Case Neglect Therapy: A Point of Theory , 1989 .

[34]  J. Ogden,et al.  The ‘Neglected’ Left Hemisphere and its Contribution to Visuospatial Neglect , 1987 .

[35]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Eye-movement strategy and tactics in word recognition and reading. , 1987 .

[36]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  The Posterior Parietal Area as a Spatial Generator , 1985 .

[37]  E. Renzi Disorders of space exploration and cognition , 1982 .

[38]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[39]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[40]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .