Resisting Good News: Reactions to Breast Cancer Risk Communication

Many women overestimate their percentage risk of breast cancer, even after they have received careful estimates from health professionals. In 2 experiments with 134 young adult women, 6 variables were explored that might influence such risk perception persistence. In Study 1, each of the following explanations was unrelated to persistence: public commitment, self-consistency, and unique causal risk models. In Study 2, two individual difference measures, pessimism and differences in understanding percentages, were unrelated to risk perception persistence. However, providing a "risk anchor" based on downward social comparison processes resulted in better risk acceptance at posttest that persisted at a 2-week follow-up assessment. This article discusses why comparison anchors might be important in risk feedback situations and concludes with recommendations for professionals who wish to provide accurate risk information and have patients adopt that information.

[1]  P. Harris,et al.  The illusion of control and optimism about health: on being less at risk but no more in control than others. , 1994, The British journal of social psychology.

[2]  K. Mccaul,et al.  Naive beliefs about breast cancer risk. , 1998, Women's health.

[3]  J Crocker,et al.  Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluations of others: effects of self-esteem and threat. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Ladd Wheeler,et al.  Motivation as a determinant of upward comparison , 1966 .

[5]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[6]  Lisa G. Aspinwall,et al.  Self-Affirmation Reduces Biased Processing of Health-Risk Information , 1998 .

[7]  B. Rimer,et al.  Effects of individualized breast cancer risk counseling: a randomized trial. , 1995, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[8]  A. Howell,et al.  Psychological support needs for women at high genetic risk of breast cancer: some preliminary indicators , 1998, Psycho-oncology.

[9]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Absolute and Relative Biases in Estimations of Personal Risk , 1996 .

[10]  D. Watson,et al.  Mood and the mundane: relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  C. Kiesler The psychology of commitment : experiments linking behavior to belief , 1971 .

[12]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Scales for assessing perceptions of health hazard susceptibility. , 1993, Health education research.

[13]  W M Klein Objective standards are not enough: affective, self-evaluative, and behavioral responses to social comparison information. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  W. Klein,et al.  Risk communication and worry about breast cancer , 2003, Psychology, health & medicine.

[15]  Mccaul Kd,et al.  Naive beliefs about breast cancer risk. , 1998 .

[16]  Leif D. Nelson,et al.  Do Messages about Health Risks Threaten the Self? Increasing the Acceptance of Threatening Health Messages Via Self-Affirmation , 2000 .

[17]  C. L. Gruder,et al.  Determinants of social comparison choices , 1971 .

[18]  M. Gail,et al.  Projecting individualized probabilities of developing breast cancer for white females who are being examined annually. , 1989, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[19]  Suzanne M. Miller,et al.  A randomized trial of breast cancer risk counseling: interacting effects of counseling, educational level, and coping style. , 1996, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[20]  B. Rimer,et al.  Informing Women About Their Breast Cancer Risks: Truth and Consequences , 2001, Health communication.

[21]  G. Colditz,et al.  Colon Cancer: Risk Perceptions and Risk Communication , 2004, Journal of health communication.

[22]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. , 1987 .

[23]  Michael W. Bridges,et al.  Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  T. Wills Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology , 1981 .

[25]  W. Klein,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Resistance of Personal Risk Perceptions to Debiasing Interventions , 2002 .

[26]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[28]  D. O. Sears College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology's view of human nature. , 1986 .