Task switching and shifting between stopping and going: Developmental change in between-trial control adjustments.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Crone,et al. Brain Regions Mediating Flexible Rule Use during Development , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[2] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Perseverative Behavior and Adaptive Control in Older Adults: Performance Monitoring, Rule Induction, and Set Shifting , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[3] D. Stuss,et al. Principles of frontal lobe function , 2002 .
[4] A. Minkowski,et al. Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life , 1968 .
[5] G. J. Robertson. Raven's Progressive Matrices , 2010 .
[6] F. N. Dempster,et al. The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging , 1992 .
[7] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[9] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Task switches under Go/NoGo conditions and the decomposition of switch costs , 2003 .
[10] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[11] S. Reimers,et al. Task switching across the life span: effects of age on general and specific switch costs. , 2005, Developmental psychology.
[12] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .
[13] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: rules without symbols. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[14] M. Welsh. Developmental and clinical variations in executive functions. , 2002 .
[15] M. W. van der Molen. Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: evidence from psychophysiological measures. , 2000, Biological psychology.
[16] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[17] M. Rieger,et al. Inhibition of ongoing responses following frontal, nonfrontal, and basal ganglia lesions. , 2003, Neuropsychology.
[18] Dorret I. Boomsma,et al. Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: Conflict adaptation or associative priming? , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[19] A. Allport,et al. Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.
[20] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Switching between tasks and responses: a developmental study. , 2006, Developmental science.
[21] Agnès Blaye,et al. Setting goals to switch between tasks: effect of cue transparency on children's cognitive flexibility. , 2009, Developmental psychology.
[22] U. Lindenberger,et al. Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: the role of verbalization in task preparation. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[23] J. Mazziotta,et al. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development , 1987, Annals of neurology.
[24] Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al. Brain development during puberty: state of the science. , 2006, Developmental science.
[25] Dima Amso,et al. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Beyond What Develops When Neuroimaging May Inform How Cognition Changes With Development , 2022 .
[26] Jelena Jovanovic,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[27] P. Huttenlocher. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. , 1979, Brain research.
[28] F. Karayanidis,et al. Sequence effects in cued task switching modulate response preparedness and repetition priming processes. , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[29] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.
[30] J. Raven,et al. Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .
[31] M. W. Molen. Developmental changes in inhibitory processing: evidence from psychophysiological measures , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[32] N. Chater,et al. Asymmetrical switch costs in children , 2006 .
[33] G. Logan,et al. The Development of Selective Inhibitory Control Across the Life Span , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.
[34] J. Raven,et al. Raven Progressive Matrices , 2003 .
[35] G. Logan,et al. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] J. Gabrieli,et al. Immature Frontal Lobe Contributions to Cognitive Control in Children Evidence from fMRI , 2002, Neuron.
[37] D. E. Astle,et al. Dissociating neural indices of dynamic cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: An ERP study of task switching , 2006, Brain Research.
[38] Yihong Yang,et al. A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control , 2002 .
[39] A. Kramer,et al. Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. , 2001, Developmental psychology.
[40] Conor V. Dolan,et al. Source (or Part of the following Source): Type Article Title Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis Author(s) Age-related Change in Executive Function: Developmental Trends and a Latent Variable Analysis , 2022 .
[41] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[42] Martina Rieger,et al. Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .
[43] A. Diamond. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. , 2002 .
[44] R. Johnston,et al. Recognition of Familiar Individuals in Golden Hamsters: A New Method and Functional Neuroanatomy , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] R. Woodworth,et al. Experimental psychology, Rev. ed. , 1954 .
[46] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Methods for Dealing With Reaction Time Outliers , 1992 .
[47] Kate Nation,et al. Shifting development in mid-childhood: the influence of between-task interference. , 2009, Developmental psychology.
[48] D. A. Grant,et al. A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.
[49] P. Zelazo. The development of conscious control in childhood , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[50] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[51] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[52] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[53] S. Bunge,et al. Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory and cognitive control , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[54] Ronald Hübner,et al. Response execution, selection, or activation: What is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting? , 2006, Psychological research.
[55] Robert K. Heaton,et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual – Revised and Expanded , 1993 .
[56] D. Mathalon,et al. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. , 1994, Archives of neurology.
[57] Gedeon O. Deák,et al. Effects of age, reminders, and task difficulty on young children’s rule-switching flexibility , 2004 .
[58] M. Walton,et al. The role of medial frontal cortex in making decisions and changing tasks , 2004 .
[59] M. Korkman,et al. Differential Development of Attention and Executive Functions in 3- to 12-Year-Old Finnish Children , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.
[60] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[61] E. Crone,et al. Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[62] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[63] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[64] Eliana Colunga,et al. More than a matter of getting 'unstuck': flexible thinkers use more abstract representations than perseverators. , 2009, Developmental science.
[65] Donald Laming,et al. Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .
[66] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Response selection and response execution in task switching: evidence from a go-signal paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[67] A. Vandierendonck,et al. Inhibiting responses when switching: Does it matter? , 2005, Experimental psychology.
[68] Thomas Kleinsorge,et al. Preparation for a forthcoming task is sufficient to produce subsequent shift costs , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[69] B. J. Casey,et al. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[70] T Kleinsorge,et al. Response repetition benefits and costs. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[71] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[72] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[73] M. W. Molen,et al. Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses , 2004 .
[74] R. Kliegl,et al. Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[75] F. Vargha-Khadem,et al. Maturation of action monitoring from adolescence to adulthood: an ERP study. , 2005, Developmental science.
[76] Suzanne E. Welcome,et al. Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children , 2022 .
[77] D. Amso,et al. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[78] I. Biederman,et al. Mental set and mental shift revisited , 1976 .
[79] P. Yakovlev,et al. The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .
[80] S. Monsell. Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[81] D. Molfese,et al. Developmental Variations in Learning : Applications to Social, Executive Function, Language, and Reading Skills , 2001 .
[82] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[83] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Selective stopping in task switching: The role of response selection and response execution. , 2006, Experimental psychology.
[84] J. Pekar,et al. Functional brain correlates of response time variability in children , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[85] N. Meiran,et al. Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[86] F. Craik,et al. Executive function across the life span. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[87] Alan C. Evans,et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[88] P. J. Foley. The foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1959, Canadian journal of psychology.
[89] Narayanan Srinivasan,et al. Development of task switching and post-error-slowing in children , 2009, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[90] I. Koch,et al. Linking inhibition to activation in the control of task sequences , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[91] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.