Vision without knowledge.

A brain-damaged patient (D.F.) with visual form agnosia is described and discussed. D.F. has a profound inability to recognize objects, places and people, in large part because of her inability to make perceptual discriminations of size, shape or orientation, despite having good visual acuity. Yet she is able to perform skilled actions that depend on that very same size, shape and orientation information that is missing from her perceptual awareness. It is suggested that her intact vision can best be understood within the framework of a dual processing model, according to which there are two cortical processing streams operating on different coding principles, for perception and for action, respectively. These may be expected to have different degrees of dependence on top-down information. One possibility is that D.F.'s lack of explicit awareness of the visual cues that guide her behaviour may result from her having to rely on a processing system which is not knowledge-based in a broad sense. Conversely, it may be that the perceptual system can provide conscious awareness of its products in normal individuals by virtue of the fact that it does interact with a stored base of visual knowledge.

[1]  M. Goodale,et al.  Chapter 28 Visual pathways to perception and action , 1993 .

[2]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  Parietal Visual Neurons Coding Three-Dimensional Characteristics of Objects and Their Relation to Hand Action , 1997 .

[3]  B. Bridgeman Conscious vs Unconscious Processes , 1992 .

[4]  R. Gregory Perceptions as hypotheses. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[5]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  A. Milner,et al.  A Disorder of Lightness Discrimination in a Case of Visual Form Agnosia , 1989, Cortex.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional anatomy of pointing and grasping in humans. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[9]  M. Jeannerod The formation of finger grip during prehension. A cortically mediated visuomotor pattern , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  A. Milner,et al.  Cerebral correlates of visual awareness , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[12]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[13]  R Kawashima,et al.  Changes in rCBF during grasping in humans examined by PET , 1996, Neuroreport.

[14]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Measuring time to awareness , 1991, Neuroreport.

[15]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  F. Drislane,et al.  A historic case of visual agnosia revisited after 40 years. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  A. Alavi,et al.  Clinical and positron emission tomography studies of visual apperceptive agnosia , 1996 .

[19]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  Malcolm P. Young,et al.  Objective analysis of the topological organization of the primate cortical visual system , 1992, Nature.

[22]  M. Jeannerod Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects , 1981 .

[23]  A. Milner,et al.  The perception and prehension of objects oriented in the depth plane , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  D. Benson,et al.  Visual recognition through kinaesthetic mediation , 1982, Psychological Medicine.

[27]  A Mack,et al.  Saccadic programming and perceived location. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[28]  S. Chieffi,et al.  Visual illusion and action , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[30]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[32]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[33]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  A neurological dissociation between shape from shading and shape from edges , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Cortical Visual Areas and Their Interactions , 1972 .

[35]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  M. Goodale,et al.  The use of visuomotor cues as a strategy for making perceptual judgments in a patient with visual form agnosia. , 1996 .

[38]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A kinematic analysis of reaching and grasping movements in a patient recovering from optic ataxia , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  R. Gregory,et al.  Knowledge in perception and illusion. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  R. Efron,et al.  What is Perception , 1969 .

[42]  A. Milner,et al.  Visuomotor sensitivity for shape and orientation in a patient with visual form agnosia , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Milner Ad,et al.  Copying without perceiving: motor imagery in visual form agnosia. , 1997 .

[44]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Impairment of grasping movements following a bilateral posterior parietal lesion , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  J. Bullier,et al.  Anatomical segregation of two cortical visual pathways in the macaque monkey , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.