The Mere Exposure Effect Is Differentially Sensitive to Different Judgment Tasks
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. F. Barrett,et al. Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .
[2] R. Zajonc,et al. Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[3] R. Bauer,et al. Implicit learning of new faces in prosopagnosia: An application of the mere-exposure paradigm , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[4] R. Zajonc,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. , 1980, Science.
[5] R. Zajonc. Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .
[6] Richard S. Lazarus,et al. On the Primacy of Cognition. , 1984 .
[7] John G. Seamon,et al. The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affect judgments. , 1995 .
[8] N. Cohen,et al. The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection , 1988, Brain and Cognition.
[9] A. Reber. An evolutionary context for the cognitive unconscious , 1992 .
[10] R. Zajonc,et al. Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[11] R. Bornstein. Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. , 1989 .
[12] Robert F. Bornstein,et al. The Generalizability of Subliminal Mere Exposure Effects: Influence of Stimuli Perceived Without Awareness on Social Behavior , 1987 .
[13] S. Corkin,et al. Intact priming of patterns despite impaired memory , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[14] R. Zajonc. On the primacy of affect. , 1984 .
[15] N. Brody,et al. Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[16] J. Seamon,et al. A mere exposure effect for transformed three-dimensional objects: Effects of reflection, size, or color changes on affect and recognition , 1997, Memory & cognition.
[17] J M VANDERPLAS,et al. The association value of random shapes. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[18] Daniel L. Schacter,et al. Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: effects of size and reflection. , 1992 .
[19] D. Schacter. Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .
[20] G. Mandler. Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. , 1980 .
[21] George Mandler,et al. Nonspecific Effects of Exposure on Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 1987 .
[22] R. Zajonc. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .
[23] Eyal M. Reingold,et al. Comparing Direct (Explicit) and Indirect (Implicit) Measures to Study Unconscious Memory , 1991 .
[24] P. Niedenthal,et al. The heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention. , 1994 .
[25] Mortimer Mishkin,et al. Chapter 12 – THE AMYGDALA: SENSORY GATEWAY TO THE EMOTIONS , 1986 .
[26] John G. Seamon,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: effects of shadowing, masking, and cerebral laterality , 1983 .
[27] R. Bauer,et al. Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test , 1984, Neuropsychologia.
[28] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Do alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome patients acquire affective reactions? , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[29] M. Gazzaniga. Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience , 1984, Springer US.
[30] A. Reber. The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective , 1992, Consciousness and Cognition.
[31] G. Mandler,et al. Knowing and liking , 1983 .
[32] Daniel L. Schacter,et al. Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information , 1995, Nature.
[33] D. Kelly,et al. Contemporary Indications for Psychosurgery , 1981, British Journal of Psychiatry.
[34] L. Cooper,et al. Implicit memory for unfamiliar objects depends on access to structural descriptions , 1990 .
[35] D. V. von Cramon,et al. A contribution to the anatomical basis of thalamic amnesia. , 1985, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[36] R. M. Bauer. Visual hypoemotionality as a symptom of visual-limbic disconnection in man. , 1982, Archives of neurology.
[37] Suzanne Corkin,et al. ACQUISITION OF MOTOR SKILL AFTER BILATERAL MEDIAL TEMPORAL-LOBE EXCISION , 1968 .
[38] A. Greenwald,et al. Preferences need no inferences?: The cognitive basis of unconscious mere exposure effects. , 1994 .
[39] F M Miezin,et al. Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[40] N. Brody,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: II. Effect of delay between study and test , 1983 .
[41] A. Harrison,et al. Response competition, frequency, exploratory behavior, and liking. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[42] Andrew R. Mayes,et al. Location of Lesions in Korsakoff's Syndrome: Neuropsychological and Neuropathological Data on Two Patients , 1988, Cortex.
[43] James L. McGaugh,et al. The amygdala and emotional memory , 1995, Nature.
[44] L. Squire. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.
[45] L. Jacoby,et al. Memory influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. , 1988 .
[46] W. Wilson,et al. Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning. , 1979 .
[47] Joseph E LeDoux. Cognition and emotion: Processing functions and brain systems , 1984 .
[48] L. Squire,et al. Independence of memory functions and emotional behavior: Separate contributions of the hippocampal formation and the amygdala , 1991, Hippocampus.
[49] L. Cermak,et al. Alcoholic Korsakoff's Syndrome: An Information-Processing Approach to Amnesia , 1980 .
[50] R. Bornstein,et al. Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[51] B. Milner,et al. Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M.☆ , 1968 .
[52] C. W. Hughes. Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience , 1982 .
[53] G. Bonanno,et al. Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposures to random geometric shapes. , 1986, The American journal of psychology.
[54] D. Stang. Effects of "mere exposure" on learning and affect. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[55] JANOS KURUCZ,et al. Prosopo‐Affective Agnosia Associated with Chronic Organic Brain Syndrome , 1979, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.
[56] M. Oscar-Berman. Neuropsychological consequences of long-term chronic alcoholism. , 1980, American scientist.
[57] Michael D. Kopelman,et al. Handbook of memory disorders , 1995 .
[58] S. Corkin. Lasting Consequences of Bilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy: Clinical Course and Experimental Findings in H.M. , 1984 .
[59] L. Jacoby,et al. On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[60] D. Schacter. Understanding implicit memory. A cognitive neuroscience approach. , 1992, The American psychologist.
[61] M W Matlin,et al. Response competition, recognition, and affect. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.