Behavioral inhibition: stability and associations with adaptation from childhood to early adulthood.

The stability of individual differences in behavioral inhibition and their association with peer relations, emotional distress, and life-course timing were examined in a longitudinal study of 205 individuals from childhood (ages 8 to 12) to early adulthood (ages 17 to 24). Behavioral inhibition was conceptualized as stranger wariness and measured through ratings made by interviewers following individual interview or testing sessions. Individual differences in behavioral inhibition were consistent from childhood to early adulthood (r = .57). In early adulthood, higher behavioral inhibition was associated with a less positive, less active social life for both sexes and, for men, with greater emotional distress and negative emotionality. For both genders, participants who were inhibited as children were less likely to have moved away from their family of origin by the early adulthood assessment.

[1]  R. Parritz,et al.  Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: the moderating role of attachment security. , 1996, Child development.

[2]  Scott D. Gest,et al.  The structure and coherence of competence from childhood through adolescence. , 1995, Child development.

[3]  Phil A. Silva,et al.  Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young adulthood: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. , 1995, Child development.

[4]  J. Asendorpf,et al.  Traits and relationship status: stranger versus peer group inhibition and test intelligence versus peer group competence as early predictors of later self-esteem. , 1994, Child development.

[5]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Temperament and the development of personality. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[6]  A. Masten,et al.  Peer reputation in middle childhood as a predictor of adaptation in adolescence: a seven-year follow-up. , 1991, Child development.

[7]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Children's Peer Relationships: Longitudinal Prediction of Internalizing and Externalizing Problems from Middle to Late Childhood. , 1990 .

[8]  J. Asendorpf Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor model , 1990 .

[9]  J. Asendorpf Shyness as a Final Common Pathway for Two Different Kinds of Inhibition , 1989 .

[10]  A. Matheny,et al.  Children's behavioral inhibition over age and across situations: genetic similarity for a trait during change. , 1989, Journal of personality.

[11]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Sociability and social withdrawal in childhood: stability and outcomes. , 1989, Journal of personality.

[12]  J. Kagan,et al.  Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. , 1988, Child development.

[13]  A. Caspi,et al.  Moving away from the World: Life-Course Patterns of Shy Children. , 1988 .

[14]  A. Tellegen,et al.  Competence and stress in school children: the moderating effects of individual and family qualities. , 1988, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[15]  A. Masten,et al.  Humor and competence in school-aged children. , 1986, Child development.

[16]  A. Costello,et al.  Parent-child agreement on child psychiatric symptoms assessed via structured interview. , 1986, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  Ann S. Masten,et al.  A revised class play method of peer assessment. , 1985 .

[18]  D. Pellegrini,et al.  Social cognition and competence in middle childhood. , 1985, Child development.

[19]  J. Kagan,et al.  Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. , 1984 .

[20]  J. Kagan,et al.  Behavioral inhibition in young children. , 1984 .

[21]  C. Edelbrock,et al.  Manual for the Child: Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile , 1983 .

[22]  P. Pilkonis The behavioral consequences of shyness1 , 1977 .

[23]  L. Derogatis,et al.  Confirmation of the dimensional structure of the scl‐90: A study in construct validation , 1977 .

[24]  L. Robins Deviant children grown up. A sociological and Psychiatric study of sociopathic personality. The William & Wilkins Company, Baltimore 1966, XIV und 351 Seiten , 1967, Monatsschrift für Kriminologie und Strafrechtsreform.

[25]  R. Davidson,et al.  Personality and behavior in parents of temperamentally inhibited and uninhibited children , 1994 .

[26]  Nathan A. Fox,et al.  The development of emotion regulation : biological and behavioral considerations , 1994 .

[27]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Social Withdrawal, inhibition, and Shyness in Childhood , 1993 .

[28]  A. Angold,et al.  Precursors and causes in development and psychopathology , 1993 .

[29]  J. Kagan,et al.  Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder. , 1992, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[30]  J. Reznick,et al.  Perspectives on behavioral inhibition , 1989 .

[31]  J. Kagan The concept of behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. , 1989 .

[32]  R. Hinde,et al.  Relationships within families : mutual influences , 1988 .

[33]  R. Plomin,et al.  The Study of temperament : changes, continuities, and challenges , 1986 .

[34]  J. Stevenson-Hinde,et al.  Temperamental characteristics of three- to four-year-old boys and girls and child-family interactions. , 1985, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[35]  A. Tellegen,et al.  STUDIES OF STRESS-RESISTANT CHILDREN: METHODS, VARIABLES, AND PRELIMINARY FINDINGS , 1984 .

[36]  C. Lord,et al.  APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 1984 .

[37]  A. Buss Self-consciousness and social anxiety , 1980 .

[38]  E. J. Anthony,et al.  Three clinical faces of childhood , 1976 .

[39]  B. Wolman Manual of child psychopathology , 1971 .