The worked example effect, the generation effect, and element interactivity.

The worked example effect indicates that examples providing full guidance on how to solve a problem result in better test performance than a problem-solving condition with no guidance. The generation effect occurs when learners generating responses demonstrate better test performance than learners in a presentation condition that provides an answer. This contradiction may be resolved by the suggestion that the worked example effect occurs for complex, high-element interactivity materials that impose a heavy working memory load whereas the generation effect is applicable for low-element interactivity materials. Two experiments tested this hypothesis in the area of geometry instruction using students with different levels of prior knowledge in geometry. The results of Experiment 1 indicated a worked example effect obtained for materials high in element interactivity and a generation effect for materials low in element interactivity. As levels of expertise increased in Experiment 2, thus reducing effective complexity, this interaction was replaced by a generation effect for all materials. These results suggest that when students need to learn low-element interactivity material, learning will be enhanced if they generate rather than study responses but if students need to learn high-element interactivity material, study may be preferable to generating responses.

[1]  R. Craig Directed versus independent discovery of established relations. , 1956 .

[2]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[3]  J. Sweller,et al.  Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. , 1987 .

[4]  H. Simon,et al.  A simulation of memory for chess positions. , 1973 .

[5]  Norman J. Slamucka,et al.  The generation effect when generation fails , 1983 .

[6]  Richard E. Clark,et al.  Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching , 2006 .

[7]  J. Sweller,et al.  The Use of Worked Examples as a Substitute for Problem Solving in Learning Algebra , 1985 .

[8]  J. Sweller,et al.  Natural Information Processing Systems , 2006 .

[9]  F. Craik,et al.  Depth of processing and the retention of words , 1975 .

[10]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[11]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  The generation effect as an artifact of selective displaced rehearsal. , 1987 .

[12]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. , 2001 .

[13]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  The Expertise Reversal Effect , 2003 .

[14]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[15]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[16]  Farhad Kazemi,et al.  Comparison of Problem-Based Learning Approach and Traditional Teaching on Attitude, Misconceptions and Mathematics Performance of University Students , 2012 .

[17]  R. Mayer Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. , 2004, The American psychologist.

[18]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory , 2020, Encyclopedia of Education and Information Technologies.

[19]  S. Tyler,et al.  Cognitive Effort and Memory , 1979 .

[20]  Hyun-Jeong Lee,et al.  Measuring cognitive load with electroencephalography and self-report: focus on the effect of English-medium learning for Korean students , 2014 .

[21]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent Developments and Future Directions , 2005 .

[22]  E. Keislar,et al.  Learning by discovery : a critical appraisal , 1969 .

[23]  Sharon K Tindall-Ford,et al.  When two sensory modes are better than one , 1997 .

[24]  J. Sweller Element Interactivity and Intrinsic, Extraneous, and Germane Cognitive Load , 2010 .

[25]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  L. A. McElroy,et al.  Memorial consequences of generating nonwords: Implications for semantic-memory interpretations of the generation effect , 1982 .

[27]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Expertise in Problem Solving. , 1981 .

[28]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out-Examples: A Study on Individual Differences , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[29]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  Perception and memory in chess: Heuristics of the professional eye , 1996 .

[30]  J. Sweller,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge and Why Teaching Generic Skills Does Not Work , 2014 .

[31]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[32]  D. Griffith,et al.  The attentional demands of mnemonic control processes , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[33]  Michael A. McDaniel,et al.  The generation effect: A meta-analytic review , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Vincent Aleven,et al.  The worked-example effect: Not an artefact of lousy control conditions , 2009, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[35]  Carl E. McFarland,et al.  Retrieval of internally versus externally generated words in episodic memory , 1980 .

[36]  Fred G. W. C. Paas,et al.  The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures , 1992 .

[37]  J. Sweller COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY, LEARNING DIFFICULTY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN , 1994 .

[38]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Long-term working memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[39]  S. Derry,et al.  Learning from Examples: Instructional Principles from the Worked Examples Research , 2000 .

[40]  Danielle S. McNamara,et al.  A Procedural Explanation of the Generation Effect for Simple and Difficult Multiplication Problems and Answers , 2000 .

[41]  P. Chandler,et al.  Why Some Material Is Difficult to Learn , 1994 .

[42]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[43]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Effects of decision difficulty on recognition and recall , 1979 .

[44]  L. Steffe,et al.  Constructivism in education. , 1995 .

[45]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Short-term retention of individual verbal items. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  D. Ausubel,et al.  Some psychological and educational limitations of learning by discovery , 2016 .

[47]  N. J. Slamecka,et al.  The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon , 1978 .