On inferring one's beliefs from one's attempt and consequences for subsequent compliance.

D. J. Bem (1967, 1972) has suggested that a person may infer his or her beliefs from his or her actions. With his information-processing viewpoint, D. J. Bem proposed that individuals, by observing their past behaviors, may draw information for assessing their beliefs about themselves. There is a question, however, about the mechanism of self-perception when there is inconsistency between one's attempt to realize an intended goal and the outcome of the action. In a series of field studies, participants who had unsuccessfully tried to help a stranger were more willing to comply with a relatively large request made later. Implications for self-perception theory as well as for enhancing susceptibility to influence techniques are discussed.

[1]  Grzegorz Sedek,et al.  Personal control in action : cognitive and motivational mechanisms , 1998 .

[2]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Preference for Consistency: The Development of a Valid Measure and the Discovery of Surprising Behavioral Implications , 1995 .

[3]  D. Gorassini,et al.  Does self-perception change explain the foot-in-the-door effect? , 1995 .

[4]  R. Cialdini Influence: Science and practice, 3rd ed. , 1993 .

[5]  J. Laird,et al.  The process of emotional experience: A self-perception theory. , 1992 .

[6]  J. Dillard The Current Status of Research on Sequential-Request Compliance Techniques , 1991 .

[7]  M. Kofta,et al.  When cognitive exertion does not yield cognitive gain: toward an informational explanation of learned helplessness. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  J. Dillard Self-Inference and the Foot-in-the-Door Technique Quantity of Behavior and Attitudinal Mediation , 1990 .

[9]  M. Kofta,et al.  Repeated failure: a source of helplessness or a factor irrelevant to its emergence? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Feeling and facial efference: implications of the vascular theory of emotion. , 1989, Psychological review.

[11]  F. Strack,et al.  Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  F. M. Haemmerlie,et al.  Self-perception theory, salience of behavior, and a control-enhancing program for the elderly , 1987 .

[13]  Kent B. Monroe,et al.  Effectiveness of Multiple Request Strategies: A Synthesis of Research Results , 1986 .

[14]  S. Andersen,et al.  Salience and Self-Inference: The Role of Biased Recollections in Self-Inference Processes , 1986 .

[15]  John E. Hunter,et al.  Sequential-request persuasive strategies: Meta-analysis of foot-in-the-door and door-in-the-face. , 1984 .

[16]  R. Cialdini Influence: Science and Practice , 1984 .

[17]  S. Andersen Self-knowledge and social inference: II. The diagnosticity of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. , 1984 .

[18]  L. Ross,et al.  Self-knowledge and social inference: I. The impact of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. , 1984 .

[19]  A. L. Beaman,et al.  Fifteen Years of Foot-in-the Door Research , 1983 .

[20]  W. DeJong,et al.  External Pressure to Comply , 1982 .

[21]  Steven J. Gilbert Another Look at the Milgram Obedience Studies , 1981 .

[22]  R. H. Rittle Changes in Helping Behavior , 1981 .

[23]  M. Goldman,et al.  Compliance Employing a Two-Feet-in-the-Door Procedure , 1981 .

[24]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Undermining Effects of the Foot‐in‐the‐Door Technique with Extrinsic Rewards , 1979 .

[25]  W. DeJong An examination of self-perception mediation of the foot-in-the-door effect. , 1979 .

[26]  Jerome B. Kernan,et al.  Compliance with an Interview Request: A Foot-in-the-Door, Self-Perception Interpretation , 1977 .

[27]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[28]  R. Cialdini,et al.  Test of a concession procedure for inducing verbal, behavioral, and further compliance with a request to give blood. , 1976, The Journal of applied psychology.

[29]  Clive Seligman,et al.  Relationship between compliance in the foot-in-the-door paradigm and size of first request. , 1976 .

[30]  C. Seligman,et al.  Compliance in the foot-in-the-door technique as a function of issue similarity and persuasion , 1976 .

[31]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[32]  D. Bem Self-Perception Theory , 1972 .

[33]  D. McMillen,et al.  Transgression, self-image, and compliant behavior. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  Anthony G. Greenwald,et al.  The open-mindedness of the counterattitudinal role player , 1969 .

[35]  J. Mills,et al.  Effect of Public Commitment upon Opinions. , 1969 .

[36]  R. Nisbett,et al.  On inferring one's beliefs from one's behavior. , 1969 .

[37]  A. Gross,et al.  Some effects of guilt on compliance. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  R. Decharms Personal causation : the internal affective determinants of behavior , 1968 .

[39]  J. Freedman,et al.  Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. , 1967 .

[40]  D. Bem Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. , 1967, Psychological review.

[41]  H. Kelley Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .

[42]  J. Freedman,et al.  Compliance without pressure: the foot-in-the-door technique. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  E. Sadalla,et al.  Behavioral consequences of transgression: I. The effects of social recognition. , 1966 .

[44]  N. Lewis A dynamic theory of personality , 1935 .