Behavioral constraints in the development of neuronal properties: a cortical model embedded in a real-world device.

The ability of organisms to categorize diverse and often novel stimuli depends on ongoing interactions with their environment. In a modality such as vision, categorization requires the generation of both selective and invariant responses of cortical neurons to complex visual stimuli. How does behavior contribute to shaping the responses of these neurons? Analysis of this question is made difficult by the complex multilevel interactions between many neural and behavioral variables. To mitigate this difficulty, we studied the development and ongoing plasticity of pattern-selective neuronal responses by means of synthetic neural modeling. For this purpose, we constructed Darwin V, which consists of a simulated neuronal model embedded in a real-world device that is capable of motion and autonomous behavior. The neuronal model consists of four major components: a visual system (containing cortical and subcortical networks); a taste system based on conductance; sets of motor neurons capable of triggering behavior; and a diffuse ascending (value) system. The modeled visual cortex consists of two areas: a topographic map responsive to elementary features connected to a higher-order map composed of initially non-selective neuronal units. During behavior over time in its environment, Darwin V encounters numerous objects consisting of black metal cubes displaying different patterns of white blobs and stripes. Initially, the lack of specific higher-order visual responses does not allow visual pattern discrimination, and appetitive and aversive behaviors are triggered by the 'taste' (surface conductivity of objects) alone. In the course of sensory experience, however, changes occur in visual and sensorimotor connection strengths, with two major consequences. First, units within the higher visual area acquire responses that are both pattern selective and translation invariant. Second, as a result of the operation of the value system, these responses become linked to appropriate behaviors. Analysis of Darwin V after such changes indicates that the continuity of self-generated movements is essential for the development of pattern-selective and translation-invariant responses. The concomitant development of a preference for foveal over parafoveal objects was found to be due to increased behavioral interactions with object cubes gripped by the centrally mounted effector (snout) of Darwin V. Finally, even after development of higher-order visual responses, visual responses to more frequently encountered objects continued to be enhanced, while other responses were diminished. Overall, the detailed study of Darwin V over multiple levels of organization provides a heuristically revealing example of the crucial role played by behavioral and environmental interactions in the development of complex responses by specialized neurons.

[1]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Innate and environmental factors in the development of the kitten's visual cortex. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  C. Gross 7 – The Neural Basis of Stimulus Equivalence Across Retinal Translation , 1977 .

[4]  D. N. Spinelli,et al.  Plasticity: the mirror of experience. , 1979, Science.

[5]  E. Bienenstock,et al.  Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  M. Cynader,et al.  Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[8]  R. J. Herrnstein,et al.  Riddles of natural categorization , 1985 .

[9]  M. Johnson,et al.  Effects of early experience on the development of filial preferences in the domestic chick. , 1985, Developmental psychobiology.

[10]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[11]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortexYasushi Miyashita , 1988, Nature.

[12]  G. Edelman,et al.  Signal and sense : local and global order in perceptual maps , 1990 .

[13]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Synthetic neural modeling: the 'Darwin' series of recognition automata , 1990, Proc. IEEE.

[14]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Functional reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in adult owl monkeys after behaviorally controlled tactile stimulation. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  G. Reeke,et al.  Selectionist models of perceptual and motor systems and implications for functionalist theories of brain function , 1990 .

[16]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Repetitive microstimulation alters the cortical representation of movements in adult rats. , 1990, Somatosensory & motor research.

[17]  Peter Földiák,et al.  Learning Invariance from Transformation Sequences , 1991, Neural Comput..

[18]  J. Kaas Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  G Tononi,et al.  Modeling perceptual grouping and figure-ground segregation by means of active reentrant connections. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  W. Singer,et al.  Developmental changes in the susceptibility to long-term potentiation of neurones in rat visual cortex slices. , 1991, Brain research. Developmental brain research.

[22]  Minami Ito,et al.  Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 1992, Nature.

[23]  E T Rolls,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b owl monkeys trained in a frequency-discrimination task. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  G. Edelman,et al.  Reentry and the problem of integrating multiple cortical areas: simulation of dynamic integration in the visual system. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  C. Gross,et al.  Representation of visual stimuli in inferior temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  G. Edelman,et al.  Synthetic neural modeling applied to a real-world artifact. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  C. Gross,et al.  Response properties of neurons in temporal cortical visual areas of infant monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  G. Reeke,et al.  Behaviorally based modeling and computational approaches to neuroscience. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  P M Gochin Properties of simulated neurons from a model of primate inferior temporal cortex. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Learning‐related Alterations in the Visual Responsiveness of Neurons in a Memory System of the Chick Brain , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  A. Mikami,et al.  Visual response properties of single neurons in the temporal pole of behaving monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Tovée,et al.  Translation invariance in the responses to faces of single neurons in the temporal visual cortical areas of the alert macaque. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  H. Rodman Development of inferior temporal cortex in the monkey. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  Mark F. Bear,et al.  Co-regulation of long-term potentiation and experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in visual cortex by age and experience , 1995, Nature.

[38]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Development and plasticity of the neural circuitry underlying visual recognition memory. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[39]  K. Fox,et al.  The critical period for long-term potentiation in primary sensory cortex , 1995, Neuron.

[40]  Minami Ito,et al.  Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  Intersensory experience and early perceptual development: Attenuated prenatal sensory stimulation affects postnatal auditory and visual responsiveness in bobwhite quail chicks (Colinus virginianus). , 1995 .

[42]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[43]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representations in the primate. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Optical Imaging of Functional Organization in the Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex , 1996, Science.

[45]  I Fujita,et al.  Intrinsic connections in the macaque inferior temporal cortex , 1996, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[46]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[47]  Peter Földiák,et al.  Learning generalisation and localisation: Competition for stimulus type and receptive field , 1996, Neurocomputing.

[48]  A. Clark Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again , 1996 .

[49]  G. Edelman,et al.  A model of color vision based on cortical reentry. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[51]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Origins and early development of perception, action, and representation. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[52]  P. Churchland,et al.  The Mind-Brain Continuum: Sensory Processes , 1996 .

[53]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Neural representations, experience, and change. , 1996 .

[54]  G. Spinozzi Categorization in monkeys and chimpanzees , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[55]  Randall D. Beer,et al.  The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[56]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  A Model of Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System: Learning Rules, Activation Functions, Lateral Inhibition, and Information-Based Performance Measures , 2000, Neural Computation.