Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space

This study explored whether functional properties of the hand and tools influence the allocation of spatial attention. In four experiments that used a visual-orienting paradigm with predictable lateral cues, hands or tools were placed near potential target locations. Results showed that targets appearing in the hand’s grasping space (i.e., near the palm) and the rake’s raking space (i.e., near the prongs) produced faster responses than did targets appearing to the back of the hand, to the back of the rake, or near the forearm. Validity effects were found regardless of condition in all experiments, but they did not interact with the target-in-grasping/raking-space bias. Thus, the topology of the facilitated space around the hand is, in part, defined by the hand’s grasping function and can be flexibly extended by functional experience using a tool. These findings are consistent with the operation of bimodal neurons, and this embodied component is incorporated into a neurally based model of spatial attention.

[1]  Catherine L. Reed,et al.  The Influence of the Body and Action on Spatial Attention , 2008, WAPCV.

[2]  Catherine L. Reed,et al.  Trunk Orientation Induces Neglect-Like Lateral Biases in Covert Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[3]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  Attention and Action , 2001 .

[4]  C. Reed,et al.  Walking reveals trunk orientation bias for visual attention , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Egocentric body-centered coordinates modulate visuomotor performance , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  M. Posner,et al.  How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  C. Gross,et al.  The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons , 1995 .

[9]  Matthew Rizzo,et al.  Spatial attention: normal processes and their breakdown. , 2003, Neurologic clinics.

[10]  F. Pavani,et al.  Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[12]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  9 A map of complex movements in motor cortex of primates , 2004 .

[13]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Sensorimotor integration in the precentral gyrus: polysensory neurons and defensive movements. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[15]  D. Lindsley,et al.  25 – Brain Mechanisms of Attention and Perception1 , 1980 .

[16]  Giuseppe di Pellegrino,et al.  Neuropsychological Evidence of an Integrated Visuotactile Representation of Peripersonal Space in Humans , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Jefferson D. Grubb,et al.  Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  A Farnè,et al.  Dynamic size‐change of hand peripersonal space following tool use , 2000, Neuroreport.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  J. Duncan EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  C. Gross,et al.  Mapping Space With Neurons , 1994 .

[23]  ● Pytorch,et al.  Attention! , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  A Chatterjee,et al.  Crossmodal and sensorimotor integration in tactile awareness. , 1999, Neurology.

[26]  M S Graziano,et al.  A System of Multimodal Areas in the Primate Brain , 2001, Neuron.

[27]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Visual attention and cortical circuits , 2001 .

[28]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[29]  C. Spence,et al.  Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans , 2002, Cognition.

[30]  F. Previc The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.