The development of first graders' reading skill as a function of pre-school motivational orientation and phonemic awareness

This study examined the development of reading skill during the first school year. The predictors were pre-school motivational orientations, coping tendencies, knowledge of the alphabet and phonemic awareness. From 151 pre-school children (6 years of age) rated by pre-school teachers on motivational orientations, 32 non-readers were allocated according to their dominating motivational disposition to one of the following extreme groups: task orientation, social dependence, ego-defensive, and multiple non-task-oriented. Each group included 8 children. The subjects' phonemic awareness and knowledge of the alphabet were assessed. Coping strategies were observed in pre-school during a play-like construction task comprising three induced pressure episodes. At the end of the first grade the children were assessed on word reading skill and again on motivational orientation. Both group comparisons and idiographic analyses were made. Results indicated that pre-school phonemic awareness was associated with first grade word reading skill. Rated task orientation in pre-school enhanced significantly the prediction of fluent word reading. Task-oriented children showed significantly better word reading skill than ego-defensive or multiple non-task-oriented children. Ego-defensive and multiple non-task-oriented subjects showed significantly less task oriented, and more ego-defensive, coping behaviour under pressure than task-oriented subjects. This finding suggests greater vulnerability of ego-defensive and multiple non-task-oriented children which may contribute to diverging reading careers. Idiographic analyses indicated parallel developmental changes in reading skill, motivational orientations and coping patterns confirming the role of motivation in the formation of an individual's reading career.RésuméLes auteurs étudient le développement de la compétence en lecture pendant la première année d'école primaire (7 ans). Les facteurs prédictifs retenus ont été les orientations de la motivation, l'engagement dans la situation, la connaissance de l'alphabet et la conscience phonologique à l'école maternelle. Parmi les 151 enfants de 6 ans évalués par leurs maîtresse sur les orientations de leur motivations, 32 non-lecteurs ont été sélectionnés afin de constituer quatre groupes contrastés de huit enfants chacun selon leurs dispositions dominantes: 1) orientation vers la tâche, 2) dépendance sociale, 3) défense de soi, 4) non orientation vers la tâche. La conscience phonologique et la connaissance de l'alphabet ont été évaluées. Les stratégies d'engagement (coping) ont été caractérisées à partir d'observations à l'école maternelle pendant un jeu de construction (LEGO) comprenant trois épisodes avec des stimulations pressantes de la part de l'expérimenteur. A la fin de la première année primaire, on a évalué la compétence en lecture des enfants et de nouveau les orientations de leur motivation. Les résultats indiquent l'existence d'une liaison entre la conscience phonologique à 6 ans et la compétence à déchiffrer en fin de première année primaire. Le déchiffrage des enfants du groupe “orienté vers la tâche” a été bien meilleur que celui des enfants du groupe “défensif” et “non orienté vers la tâche”. Les enfants de ces deux derniers groupes ont fait preuve de beaucoup moins d'orientation vers la tâche et de davantage de conduites de défense dans leurs stratégies d'adaptation. Ces constats suggèrent l'existence d'une grande vulnérabilité des sujets appartenant aux groupes défensifs et non orientés vers la tâche, vulnérabilité qui peut perturber leur appentissage de la lecture. Les analyses idiographiques ont indiqué l'existence de changements développementaux parallèles en déchiffrage, orientation de la motivation et stratégie d'adaptation, confirmant ainsi le rôle de la motivation dans le déroulement de la carrière de l'apprenti lecteur.

[1]  C. Juel Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. , 1988 .

[2]  Haywood Hc,et al.  Motivational orientation of overachieving and underachieving elementary school children. , 1968 .

[3]  Nancy K. Lewkowicz Phonemic Awareness Training: What to Teach and How To Teach It. , 1980 .

[4]  J. Mckinney Longitudinal Research on the Behavioral Characteristics of Children with Learning Disabilities , 1989, Journal of learning disabilities.

[5]  J. McKinney,et al.  Classroom Behavior of Learning Disabled Children , 1982 .

[6]  R. Ryan,et al.  Self-Perceptions, Motivation, and Adjustment in Children with Learning Disabilities , 1990, Journal of learning disabilities.

[7]  G. Crombie,et al.  Children's Problem-Solving Performance: The Effects of Compliance and Cognitive Factors , 1991 .

[8]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  A Developmental Study of Learned Helplessness , 1980 .

[9]  D. Willows,et al.  Cognitive-motivational characteristics of children varying in reading ability: evidence for learned helplessness in poor readers. , 1980, Journal of educational psychology.

[10]  John G. Nicholls,et al.  Quality and Equality in Intellectual Development: The Role of Motivation in Education. , 1979 .

[11]  M. Carr,et al.  Motivational components of underachievement. , 1991 .

[12]  A. Moriarty Coping patterns of preschool children in response to intelligence test demands. , 1961, Genetic psychology monographs.

[13]  D. Gold,et al.  Compliance and problem-solving competence in girls and boys. , 1989, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[14]  Frank R. Vellutino,et al.  Phonological Coding, Phonological Awareness, and Reading Ability: Evidence from a Longitudinal and Experimental Study. , 1987 .

[15]  D. Magnusson Implications of an Interactional Paradigm for Research on Human Development , 1985 .

[16]  R. Pearl LD Children's Attributions for Success and Failure: A Replication with a Labeled LD Sample , 1982 .

[17]  S. Folkman,et al.  Stress, appraisal, and coping , 1974 .

[18]  Gail D. Heyman,et al.  Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: relationship to beliefs about goodness. , 1992, Child development.

[19]  P. Niemi Recent Explorations into the Etiology of Reading Difficulties: Insights from Language and Cognition. , 1990 .

[20]  Ingvar Lundberg,et al.  Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten , 1980 .

[21]  D. Ruble,et al.  Developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. , 1980 .

[22]  P. Salonen,et al.  Adaptation, Motivational Orientation, and Cognition in a Subnormally Performing Child: A Systemic Perspective for Training , 1992 .

[23]  Tests Guide the School Psychologist, Not the Learning Problem?. , 1995 .

[24]  Richard S. Crutchfield,et al.  Conformity and creative thinking. , 1962 .

[25]  S. Harter Effectance Motivation Reconsidered. Toward a Developmental Model , 1978 .

[26]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Socioemotional Coping and Cognitive Processes in Training Learning-Disabled Children , 1992 .

[27]  R. Lazarus,et al.  Stress-Related Transactions between Person and Environment , 1978 .

[28]  S. Harter A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components. , 1981 .

[29]  S. Harter,et al.  Trait vs. nontrait conceptualizations of intrinsic/extrinsic motivational orientation , 1992 .

[30]  M. Boekaerts,et al.  Being Concerned With Well-Being and With Learning , 1993 .

[31]  Priscilla L. Griffith,et al.  Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. , 1986 .

[32]  J. Chapman Cognitive-motivational characteristics and academic achievement of learning disabled children: A longitudinal study. , 1988 .

[33]  S. Folkman,et al.  Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer; , 1984 .

[34]  Marilyn Jager Adams,et al.  Beginning To Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. , 1991 .

[35]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Long-term development of learning activity: Motivational, cognitive, and social interaction , 1995 .

[36]  Sandra Graham,et al.  Motivational influences on cognition : task involvement, ego involvement, and depth of information processing , 1991 .

[37]  S. B. Nolen,et al.  Reasons for studying: Motivational orientations and study strategies. , 1988 .

[38]  Walter Mischel,et al.  On the interface of cognition and personality: Beyond the person–situation debate. , 1979 .

[39]  Anne E. Lincoln,et al.  Perceived competence and intrinsic motivation in learning disability children , 1979 .

[40]  Carole A. Ames,et al.  Systems of student and teacher motivation: Toward a qualitative definition. , 1984 .

[41]  B. Weiner Human Motivation: Metaphors, Theories, and Research , 1980 .

[42]  J. Werner,et al.  Two perspectives on infant color vision research , 1985 .

[43]  K. Lewin Environmental forces in child behavior and development. , 1931 .

[44]  P. Bryant,et al.  Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection , 1983, Nature.