The role of linguistic experience in the processing of probabilistic information in production
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Schreuder,et al. Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language? , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[2] P. Lieberman. Some Effects of Semantic and Grammatical Context on the Production and Perception of Speech , 1963 .
[3] Holger Mitterer,et al. How phonological reductions sometimes help the listener. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] K. Rayner,et al. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[5] Adilson E. Motter,et al. Beyond Word Frequency: Bursts, Lulls, and Scaling in the Temporal Distributions of Words , 2009, PloS one.
[6] A. Caramazza,et al. The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[7] Susan M. Garnsey,et al. Knowledge of Grammar, Knowledge of Usage: Syntactic Probabilities Affect Pronunciation Variation , 2004 .
[8] Matthew Goldrick,et al. Phonetic processing of non-native speech in semantic vs non-semantic tasks. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[9] Mark Steedman,et al. Information Structure and the Syntax-Phonology Interface , 2000, Linguistic Inquiry.
[10] George Kingsley Zipf,et al. Human behavior and the principle of least effort , 1949 .
[11] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. A processing-centered look at the contribution of givenness to durational reduction , 2012 .
[12] S. Gahl. Time and Thyme Are not Homophones: The Effect of Lemma Frequency on Word Durations in Spontaneous Speech , 2008 .
[13] M. Pickering,et al. Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[14] F.-Xavier Alario,et al. The selection of closed-class elements during language production: a reassessment of the evidence and a new look on new data , 2014 .
[15] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[16] M. Tanenhaus,et al. Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension , 2002 .
[17] M. Brysbaert,et al. The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[18] Gary S. Dell,et al. Effects of Frequency and Vocabulary Type on Phonological Speech Errors , 1990 .
[19] Albert Costa,et al. A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages , 2013, Cognition.
[20] Gary S. Dell,et al. A Synthesis of Some Recent Work in Sentence Production , 1989 .
[21] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. The Old and Thee, uh, New , 2004, Psychological science.
[22] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Synthesising meaning and processing approaches to prosody: performance matters , 2015, Language, cognition and neuroscience.
[23] Duane G. Watson,et al. Repetition is easy: Why repeated referents have reduced prominence , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[24] C. Fowler,et al. Talkers' signaling of new and old. words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction , 1987 .
[25] F. Ferreira,et al. How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums , 2002 .
[26] Dan Jurafsky,et al. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[27] G. Yeni-Komshian,et al. Age of learning effects on the duration of sentences produced in a second language , 2000 .
[28] E. Bard,et al. Controlling the Intelligibility of Referring Expressions in Dialogue , 2000 .
[29] M. Ernestus,et al. Variability in the pronunciation of non-native English the: Effects of frequency and disfluencies , 2014 .
[30] M. F. Garrett,et al. The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .
[31] M. Miozzo,et al. Which words are activated during bilingual word production? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[32] Alice Turk,et al. The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis: A Functional Explanation for Relationships between Redundancy, Prosodic Prominence, and Duration in Spontaneous Speech , 2004, Language and speech.
[33] Herbert Schriefers,et al. Selection of freestanding and bound gender-marking morphemes in speech production: a review , 2014 .
[34] J. Morgan,et al. SIGNAL TO SYNTAX : Bootstrapping From Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition , 2008 .
[35] A. Fernald,et al. Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.
[36] Duane G. Watson,et al. Reduction in prosodic prominence predicts speakers' recall: implications for theories of prosody , 2015, Language, cognition and neuroscience.
[37] Herbert Schriefers,et al. What data from the simple-picture naming task tell us about lexical competition—A rejoinder to Janssen, Schiller, and Alario , 2014 .
[38] Roger Levy,et al. Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction , 2006, NIPS.
[39] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Articulatory and lexical repetition effects on durational reduction: speaker experience vs. common ground , 2015 .
[40] Susan G. Guion,et al. Prosody in second language acquisition: Acoustic analyses of duration and F0 range , 2007 .
[41] Jason M. Brenier,et al. Predictability Effects on Durations of Content and Function Words in Conversational English , 2009 .
[42] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. THE BACON not the bacon: How children and adults understand accented and unaccented noun phrases , 2008, Cognition.
[43] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. The effects of addressee attention on prosodic prominence , 2015 .
[44] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[45] Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast , 2000 .
[46] Duane G. Watson,et al. Repetition reduction: lexical repetition in the absence of referent repetition. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[47] Jarrod Had. MCMC Methods for Multi-Response Generalized Linear Mixed Models: The MCMCglmm R Package , 2010 .
[48] N. Snider,et al. More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases , 2010 .
[49] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Audience design affects acoustic reduction via production facilitation , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[50] C. Fowler,et al. Embedded articulation: shifts in location influence speech production , 2014 .
[51] B. Munson,et al. The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[52] Ann R Bradlow,et al. Variability in Word Duration as a Function of Probability, Speech Style, and Prosody , 2009, Language and speech.
[53] Andrew Gelman,et al. General methods for monitoring convergence of iterative simulations , 1998 .
[54] Tracey M. Derwing,et al. Foreign Accent, Comprehensibility, and Intelligibility in the Speech of Second Language Learners , 1995 .
[55] D. Bates,et al. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.
[56] Janet B. Pierrehumbert,et al. Word-specific phonetics , 2001 .
[57] Kathryn Bock,et al. Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .
[58] Antje S Meyer,et al. Activation of distractor names in the picture-picture interference paradigm , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[59] Scott Seyfarth,et al. Word informativity influences acoustic duration: Effects of contextual predictability on lexical representation , 2014, Cognition.
[60] F. Alario,et al. Phonologically driven variability: the case of determiners. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[61] Melissa Baese-Berk,et al. Word durations in non-native English , 2011, J. Phonetics.
[62] P. Boersma. Praat : doing phonetics by computer (version 4.4.24) , 2006 .
[63] F.-Xavier Alario,et al. A cross-linguistic investigation of determiner production , 2001 .
[64] T. Sandoval,et al. More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[65] Kristin Lemhöfer,et al. Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English , 2011, Behavior research methods.
[66] C. Fowler. Differential Shortening of Repeated Content Words Produced in Various Communicative Contexts , 1988, Language and speech.
[67] Sang Joon Kim,et al. A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .
[68] Michele Miozzo,et al. Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[69] Yvonne Koch. The Psychology Of Learning And Motivation , 2016 .
[70] Mariapaola D'Imperio,et al. Lexical and contextual predictability: Confluent effects on the production of vowels , 2010 .
[71] William D. Raymond,et al. Probabilistic Relations between Words: Evidence from Reduction in Lexical Production , 2008 .
[72] V. Marian,et al. Repetition reduction during word and concept overlap in bilinguals. , 2015, Journal of memory and language.
[73] D. Barr,et al. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.
[74] M. Brodeur,et al. The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a New Set of 480 Normative Photos of Objects to Be Used as Visual Stimuli in Cognitive Research , 2010, PloS one.
[75] Björn Lindblom,et al. Explaining Phonetic Variation: A Sketch of the H&H Theory , 1990 .
[76] Elisabeth Selkirk,et al. The Prosodic Structure of Function Words , 2008 .
[77] Albert Costa,et al. More than words: multiword frequency effects in non-native speakers , 2016 .
[78] Duane G. Watson,et al. Why are repeated words produced with reduced durations? Evidence from inner speech and homophone production. , 2015, Journal of memory and language.
[79] A. Colomé,et al. Lexical Activation in Bilinguals' Speech Production: Language-Specific or Language-Independent? , 2001 .
[80] Sharon Goldwater,et al. Talkers account for listener and channel characteristics to communicate efficiently , 2015 .
[81] Albert Costa,et al. Characterizing the Bilingual Disadvantage in Noun Phrase Production , 2012, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[82] Paul D. Allopenna,et al. Phonological and acoustic bases for earliest grammatical category assignment: a cross-linguistic perspective , 1998, Journal of Child Language.
[83] Mirjam Ernestus,et al. Articulatory Planning Is Continuous and Sensitive to Informational Redundancy , 2005, Phonetica.
[84] Christine Fennema-Notestine,et al. Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification , 2005, Memory & cognition.