Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: Neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency

Word retrieval ability is commonly assessed with a semantic verbal fluency task, in which subjects must produce a list of exemplars of a category (e.g., animals). The order in which exemplars are produced is not random; rather, subjects tend to produce "clusters" of semantically related items (e.g., cow, pig, sheep) and occasionally "switch" to other clusters (e.g., lion, tiger, bear). Patients with frontal lobe pathology (associated with focal lesions or Parkinson's disease) exhibit reduced output on semantic fluency tasks that has been characterized as a reduction in switching, in contrast to other impaired patient groups who produce normal switches but smaller clusters (e.g., [Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Leach, L., & Freedman, M. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(2), 137-143]). The ability to initiate a switch between two semantic categories may require the selection of weakly activated representations over active (but already reported) representations. Previous studies have shown that increased demands on selection among competing representations are associated with activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and with deficits in patients with lesions including LIFG [Thompson-Schill, S. L., Jonides, J., Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., D'Esposito, M., Kan, I. P., et al. (2002). Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 109-120; Thompson-Schill, S. L., Swick, D., Farah, M. J., D'Esposito, M., Kan, I. P., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: A neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 26, 14792-14797]. In the present study, we investigated the neural correlates of switching in the verbal fluency task, and in particular, the role of the LIFG in switching between semantic sub-categories. We observed greater activation in the LIFG during switching compared to free generation (Experiment 1) and self-reported clustering (Experiment 2), which is consistent with the hypothesis that the switching mechanism is subserved by the LIFG due to high semantic selection demands.

[1]  A. Shimamura,et al.  Letter and category fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[2]  J. Jonides,et al.  Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  E E Smith,et al.  The neural substrate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  H. Rusinek,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity in a verbal fluency task , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[5]  G. Winocur,et al.  Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: the effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  John Jonides,et al.  Dissociable neural mechanisms underlying response-based and familiarity-based conflict in working memory , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  J. Hodges,et al.  Generating ‘tiger’ as an animal name or a word beginning with T: differences in brain activation , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Effect of name agreement on prefrontal activity during overt and covert picture naming , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  N. Cohen,et al.  The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  Alex Martin,et al.  Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues: A double dissociation in normal subjects using interference tasks , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  E. Perret The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  D. Perani,et al.  Functional heterogeneity of left inferior frontal cortex as revealed by fMRI , 1997, Neuroreport.

[14]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Effects of frontal lobe damage on interference effects in working memory , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: a neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  M. Freedman,et al.  Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[17]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Twenty-first century psycholinguistics : four cornerstones , 2005 .

[18]  L. Cipolotti,et al.  Dynamic aphasia: an inability to select between competing verbal responses? , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  J. Henry,et al.  A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[20]  D. Loewenstein,et al.  Cerebral metabolic effects of a verbal fluency test: a PET scan study. , 1988, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[21]  E. Cabanis,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy and Mri , 1991 .

[22]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Dissociable Controlled Retrieval and Generalized Selection Mechanisms in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[23]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[24]  S. Rourke,et al.  Verbal Fluency Component Analysis in Adults with HIV/AIDS , 2004, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[25]  C. Kremser,et al.  Brain activation pattern during a verbal fluency test in healthy male and female volunteers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  A P Shimamura,et al.  Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  A. Schleicher,et al.  Broca's region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  Tor D Wager,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  D. Abwender,et al.  Qualitative Analysis of Verbal Fluency Output: Review and Comparison of Several Scoring Methods , 2001, Assessment.

[30]  J R Hodges,et al.  Initial letter and semantic category fluency in Alzheimer's disease, Huntington's disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[31]  Anna Basso,et al.  Semantic category and initial letter word fluency in left‐brain‐damaged patients , 1997 .

[32]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Analysis of neural mechanisms underlying verbal fluency in cytoarchitectonically defined stereotaxic space—The roles of Brodmann areas 44 and 45 , 2004, NeuroImage.

[33]  J. Desmond,et al.  Dissociation of Frontal and Cerebellar Activity in a Cognitive Task: Evidence for a Distinction between Selection and Search , 1998, NeuroImage.

[34]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Cognitive resources and dual-task interference effects at retrieval in normal people: The role of the frontal lobes and medial temporal cortex. , 1994 .

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who's in control? , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Dissociations among tasks involving inhibition: A single-case study , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  G. Winocur,et al.  Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[38]  Irene P. Kan,et al.  Selection from perceptual and conceptual representations , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[40]  Semantic fluency in Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and Huntington's disease : dissociation of storage and retrieval failures , 1993 .

[41]  M. Farah,et al.  Selection ability in Alzheimer's disease: investigation of a component of semantic processing. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[42]  D. Salmon,et al.  A comparison of the category fluency deficits associated with Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease , 1989, Brain and Language.

[43]  G. Lockhead,et al.  The free recall of category examples. , 1980 .

[44]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Empirical Analyses of BOLD fMRI Statistics , 1997, NeuroImage.

[45]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Empirical analyses of BOLD fMRI statistics. I. Spatially unsmoothed data collected under null-hypothesis conditions. , 1997, NeuroImage.

[46]  K. Amunts,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: an fMRI study with clinical implications , 2002 .