CHAPTER 7 – Developing a Theory of Animal Social Learning

Over the last century, several hundred empirical studies have generated significant amounts of information about social learning in animals. While these studies have spawned a number of intuitive schemes designed to collate and categorize information on animal social learning, development of a formal theoretical framework is still in its infancy. One might expect many benefits from such a framework. Formal theory can structure and discipline thinking, tighten hypotheses, clarify mechanisms, identify key parameters, and raise questions that inspire empirical research. The field of animal social learning has not yet reached the stage of development where theoretical and empirical projects guide and inform each other. Much of the theory that does exist has been adapted from models of human culture rather than specifically designed for the analysis of social learning in other animals. This chapter attempts to outline the benefits of developing a coherent theory, reviewing and extending current theory, and pointing to the areas where further theory is required. Principal conclusion is that there is a need for greater integration of empirical and theoretical findings, so that each tradition can inform and stimulate the other.

[1]  Kenichi Aoki,et al.  A CONDITION FOR GROUP SELECTION TO PREVAIL OVER COUNTERACTING INDIVIDUAL SELECTION , 1982, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[2]  K. Laland,et al.  Social transmission of food preferences among norway rats by marking of food sites and by gustatory contact , 1993 .

[3]  K. Laland,et al.  Further experimental analysis of the social learning and transmission of foraging information amongst Norway rats , 1992, Behavioural Processes.

[4]  P. Richerson,et al.  Multiple models in social transmission of food selection by Norway rats, Rattus norvegicus , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  B. Galef Direct and Indirect Behavioral Pathways to the Social Transmission of Food Avoidance a , 1985, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[6]  P J Richerson,et al.  Group selection among alternative evolutionarily stable strategies. , 1990, Journal of theoretical biology.

[7]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: Basic postulates and a simple model , 1978 .

[8]  T. Clutton‐Brock,et al.  Mate choice on fallow deer leks , 1989, Nature.

[9]  P. Richerson,et al.  The role of evolved predispositions in cultural evolution: Or, Human Sociobiology meets Pascal's Wager , 1989 .

[10]  J. Bradbury,et al.  Mate choice in lekking sage grouse revisited: the roles of vocal display , 1991 .

[11]  Passage of time reduces effects of familiarity on social learning: functional implications , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[12]  L. Lefebvre The opening of milk bottles by birds: Evidence for accelerating learning rates, but against the wave-of-advance model of cultural transmission , 1995, Behavioural Processes.

[13]  M. Landau,et al.  Human Evolution as Narrative , 1984 .

[14]  L. Giraldeau,et al.  Scrounging prevents cultural transmission of food-finding behaviour in pigeons , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  Responses of observer rats (Rattus norvegicus) to complex, diet-related signals emitted by demonstrator rats. , 1990 .

[16]  A Piazza,et al.  Reconstruction of human evolution: bringing together genetic, archaeological, and linguistic data. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  M. Wade,et al.  Female copying and sexual selection in a marine isopod crustacean, Paracerceis sculpta , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  A SIMPLE DUAL INHERITANCE MODEL OF THE CONFLICT BETWEEN SOCIAL AND BIOLOGICAL EVOLUTION , 1976 .

[19]  Thomas Caraco,et al.  Social foraging: individual learning and cultural transmission of innovations , 1994 .

[20]  H. C. Plotkin,et al.  A multiple-level model of evolution and its implications for sociobiology , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  A. Baker,et al.  Founder effect and cultural evolution of songs in an isolated population of chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs, in the Chatham Islands , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  A. Wilson,et al.  Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolution. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  The social transmission of a food-finding technique in pigeons: what is learned? , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  L L Cavalli-Sforza,et al.  Paradox of the evolution of communication and of social interactivity. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  M J Wade,et al.  Female copying increases the variance in male mating success. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  B. Galef,et al.  A new model system for studying behavioural traditions in animals , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Alan R. Rogers,et al.  Does Biology Constrain Culture , 1988 .

[28]  A. Wilson The molecular basis of evolution. , 1985, Scientific American.

[29]  M. Lavin,et al.  Transferred flavor aversions in adult rats. , 1980, Behavioral and neural biology.

[30]  M. Feldman,et al.  Gene-culture coevolution: toward a general theory of vertical transmission. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  K. Laland,et al.  Excretory deposits surrounding food sites facilitate social learning of food preferences in Norway rats , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  B. Galef Social identification of toxic diets by Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). , 1986, Journal of comparative psychology.

[33]  P. Richerson,et al.  The Evolution of Ethnic Markers , 1987 .

[34]  E. Rogers Diffusion of Innovations , 1962 .

[35]  K. Laland A Theoretical Investigation of the Role of Social Transmission in Evolution , 1992 .

[36]  M W Feldman,et al.  Toward a theory for the evolution of cultural communication: coevolution of signal transmission and reception. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Louis Lefebvre,et al.  Cultural transmission in pigeons is affected by the number of tutors and bystanders present , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  K. Laland,et al.  Sexual selection with a culturally transmitted mating preference. , 1994, Theoretical population biology.

[39]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[40]  Fernando Nottebohm,et al.  The Origins of Vocal Learning , 1972, The American Naturalist.

[41]  B. Galef Functions of social learning about food: a causal analysis of effects of diet novelty on preference transmission , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Kevin N. Laland,et al.  Social learning and social transmission of foraging information in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) , 1990 .

[43]  Peter J. Richerson,et al.  Why does culture increase human adaptability , 1995 .

[44]  Nigel W. Bond Transferred odor aversions in adult rats. , 1982, Behavioral and neural biology.

[45]  Michael J. Wade,et al.  A Critical Review of the Models of Group Selection , 1978, The Quarterly Review of Biology.