Auditory Predictive Coding across Awareness States under Anesthesia: An Intracranial Electrophysiology Study

The systems-level mechanisms underlying loss of consciousness (LOC) under anesthesia remain unclear. General anesthetics suppress sensory responses within higher-order cortex and feedback connections, both critical elements of predictive coding hypotheses of conscious perception. Responses to auditory novelty may offer promise as biomarkers for consciousness. This study examined anesthesia-induced changes in auditory novelty responses over short (local deviant [LD]) and long (global deviant [GD]) time scales, envisioned to engage preattentive and conscious levels of processing, respectively. Electrocorticographic recordings were obtained in human neurosurgical patients (3 male, 3 female) from four hierarchical processing levels: core auditory cortex, non-core auditory cortex, auditory-related, and PFC. Stimuli were vowel patterns incorporating deviants within and across stimuli (LD and GD). Subjects were presented with stimuli while awake, and during sedation (responsive) and following LOC (unresponsive) under propofol anesthesia. LD and GD effects were assayed as the averaged evoked potential and high gamma (70–150 Hz) activity. In the awake state, LD and GD effects were present in all recorded regions, with averaged evoked potential effects more broadly distributed than high gamma activity. Under sedation, LD effects were preserved in all regions, except PFC. LOC was accompanied by loss of LD effects outside of auditory cortex. By contrast, GD effects were markedly suppressed under sedation in all regions and were absent following LOC. Thus, although the presence of GD effects is indicative of being awake, its absence is not indicative of LOC. Loss of LD effects in higher-order cortical areas may constitute an alternative biomarker of LOC. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Development of a biomarker that indexes changes in the brain upon loss of consciousness (LOC) under general anesthesia has broad implications for elucidating the neural basis of awareness and clinical relevance to mechanisms of sleep, coma, and disorders of consciousness. Using intracranial recordings from neurosurgery patients, we investigated changes in the activation of cortical networks involved in auditory novelty detection over short (local deviance) and long (global deviance) time scales associated with sedation and LOC under propofol anesthesia. Our results indicate that, whereas the presence of global deviance effects can index awareness, their loss cannot serve as a biomarker for LOC. The dramatic reduction of local deviance effects in areas beyond auditory cortex may constitute an alternative biomarker of LOC.

[1]  Anthony G. Hudetz,et al.  Bottom-Up and Top-Down Mechanisms of General Anesthetics Modulate Different Dimensions of Consciousness , 2017, Front. Neural Circuits.

[2]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[3]  James L. Bernat,et al.  Nosologic considerations in disorders of consciousness , 2017, Annals of neurology.

[4]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The proactive brain: memory for predictions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[5]  R Näätänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Christopher K. Kovach,et al.  Localization of musicogenic epilepsy to Heschl's gyrus and superior temporal plane: case report. , 2017, Journal of neurosurgery.

[7]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[8]  Daniel R. Hansen,et al.  A method for placing Heschl gyrus depth electrodes. , 2010, Journal of Neurosurgery.

[9]  C. Koch,et al.  A framework for consciousness , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Canonical Microcircuits for Predictive Coding , 2012, Neuron.

[12]  A. Yli-Hankala,et al.  Description of the Entropy™ algorithm as applied in the Datex‐Ohmeda S/5™ Entropy Module , 2004, Acta anaesthesiologica Scandinavica.

[13]  P. Schwartzkroin,et al.  Neural mechanisms. , 1994, Science.

[14]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Event related potentials elicited by violations of auditory regularities in patients with impaired consciousness , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  M. Struys,et al.  Spectral entropy measurement of patient responsiveness during propofol and remifentanil. A comparison with the bispectral index. , 2004, British journal of anaesthesia.

[16]  P. Sebel,et al.  Bispectral index monitoring allows faster emergence and improved recovery from propofol, alfentanil, and nitrous oxide anesthesia. BIS Utility Study Group. , 1997, Anesthesiology.

[17]  Kirill V Nourski,et al.  Auditory processing in the human cortex: An intracranial electrophysiology perspective , 2017, Laryngoscope investigative otolaryngology.

[18]  Peter S. Sebel,et al.  Bispectral Index Monitoring Allows Faster Emergence and Improved Recovery from Propofol, Alfentanil, and Nitrous Oxide Anesthesia , 1997 .

[19]  R. Llinás,et al.  The neuronal basis for consciousness. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  David Janssen,et al.  Cerebral responses to local and global auditory novelty under general anesthesia , 2016, NeuroImage.

[21]  Kirill V. Nourski,et al.  Processing of auditory novelty across the cortical hierarchy: An intracranial electrophysiology study , 2018, NeuroImage.

[22]  K. Leslie,et al.  Dreaming and Electroencephalographic Changes during Anesthesia Maintained with Propofol or Desflurane , 2009, Anesthesiology.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Connectivity Changes Underlying Spectral EEG Changes during Propofol-Induced Loss of Consciousness , 2012 .

[24]  G. Mashour,et al.  Prevention of intraoperative awareness in a high-risk surgical population. , 2011, The New England journal of medicine.

[25]  G. Tononi,et al.  Role of Somatostatin-Positive Cortical Interneurons in the Generation of Sleep Slow Waves , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown in cortical effective connectivity during midazolam-induced loss of consciousness , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Jakob Hohwy,et al.  Reforming the taxonomy in disorders of consciousness , 2017, Annals of neurology.

[28]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[29]  Hiroki R. Hayama,et al.  Returning from Oblivion: Imaging the Neural Core of Consciousness , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Hearing speech sounds: Top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception , 2007, Hearing Research.

[31]  R. Malach,et al.  Diminished Auditory Responses during NREM Sleep Correlate with the Hierarchy of Language Processing , 2016, PloS one.

[32]  G. Mashour Cognitive unbinding: A neuroscientific paradigm of general anesthesia and related states of unconsciousness , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[33]  Stefan Mihalas,et al.  A Comparison of Visual Response Properties in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus and Primary Visual Cortex of Awake and Anesthetized Mice , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Devor,et al.  Reversible analgesia, atonia, and loss of consciousness on bilateral intracerebral microinjection of pentobarbital , 2001, Pain.

[35]  Y. Fishman Erratum to: The Mechanisms and Meaning of the Mismatch Negativity , 2013, Brain Topography.

[36]  Jin Liu,et al.  Electroencephalographic Bispectral Index Correlates with Intraoperative Recall and Depth of Propofol-Induced Sedation , 1997, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[37]  T. Guo,et al.  The sedative component of anesthesia is mediated by GABAA receptors in an endogenous sleep pathway , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  S. Kochen,et al.  Expectation and Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Prediction , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Chernik,et al.  Validity and Reliability of the Observer's: Assessment of Alertness/Sedation Scale Study with Intravenous Midazolam , 1990, Journal of clinical psychopharmacology.

[40]  Mitchell Steinschneider,et al.  Electrocorticographic delineation of human auditory cortical fields based on effects of propofol anesthesia , 2017, NeuroImage.

[41]  Edward C. Harding,et al.  Neuronal ensembles sufficient for recovery sleep and the sedative actions of α2 adrenergic agonists , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Howard,et al.  Invasive recordings in the human auditory cortex. , 2015, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[44]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[45]  I. Winkler,et al.  Auditory processing that leads to conscious perception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by the mismatch negativity and related responses. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[46]  Bryan M. Krause,et al.  Preferential effect of isoflurane on top-down vs. bottom-up pathways in sensory cortex , 2014, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[47]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[49]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Propofol disrupts functional interactions between sensory and high‐order processing of auditory verbal memory , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[50]  G. Tononi,et al.  Breakdown of Cortical Effective Connectivity During Sleep , 2005, Science.

[51]  N. Franks General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[52]  Christopher K. Kovach,et al.  The demodulated band transform , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[53]  J. Armony,et al.  Auditory Processing across the Sleep-Wake Cycle Simultaneous EEG and fMRI Monitoring in Humans , 2000, Neuron.

[54]  Scott A. Love,et al.  A practical guide for the identification of major sulcogyral structures of the human cortex , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.

[55]  D. Mumford On the computational architecture of the neocortex , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[56]  D Mumford,et al.  On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. , 1992, Biological cybernetics.

[57]  J. Hillenbrand,et al.  Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[58]  C. Koch,et al.  Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  George A Mashour,et al.  Integrating the Science of Consciousness and Anesthesia , 2006, Anesthesia and analgesia.

[60]  Y. Fishman The Mechanisms and Meaning of the Mismatch Negativity , 2014, Brain topography.

[61]  Bryan M. Krause,et al.  Disruption of cortical network activity by the general anaesthetic isoflurane , 2017, British journal of anaesthesia.

[62]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .

[63]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Disruption of hierarchical predictive coding during sleep , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  C. Koch,et al.  Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[65]  Michael B. Calford,et al.  Monaural inhibition in cat auditory cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  G. Tononi,et al.  Consciousness and Anesthesia , 2008, Science.

[67]  E. Kharasch,et al.  Bispectral Index Monitoring during Sedation with Sevoflurane, Midazolam, and Propofol , 2001, Anesthesiology.

[68]  S. Butler,et al.  Effect of propofol anaesthesia on the event-related potential mismatch negativity and the auditory-evoked potential N1. , 2002, British journal of anaesthesia.

[69]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[70]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature , 2010, NeuroImage.

[71]  B. Antkowiak,et al.  Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[72]  Bob McMurray,et al.  Functional organization of human auditory cortex: Investigation of response latencies through direct recordings , 2014, NeuroImage.

[73]  S. Marinakis,et al.  Experimental and theoretical approaches , 2015 .

[74]  Role of somatostatin-positive cortical interneurons in the generation of sleep slow waves , 2017 .

[75]  S. Shushruth Exploring the Neural Basis of Consciousness through Anesthesia , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Cortical Processing of Complex Auditory Stimuli during Alterations of Consciousness with the General Anesthetic Propofol , 2006, Anesthesiology.

[77]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[78]  S. Koelsch,et al.  The effects of anesthetics on brain activity and cognitive function , 2005, Current opinion in anaesthesiology.

[79]  C J Fiebach,et al.  Sequential effects of propofol on functional brain activation induced by auditory language processing: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2004, British journal of anaesthesia.

[80]  D. Ridder,et al.  From sensation to percept: The neural signature of auditory event-related potentials , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[81]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[82]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Attention and mismatch negativity. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[83]  G. Tononi,et al.  Auditory responses and stimulus-specific adaptation in rat auditory cortex are preserved across NREM and REM sleep. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[84]  T. Hothorn,et al.  Simultaneous Inference in General Parametric Models , 2008, Biometrical journal. Biometrische Zeitschrift.

[85]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[86]  Mitchell Steinschneider,et al.  Spectrotemporal analysis of evoked and induced electroencephalographic responses in primary auditory cortex (A1) of the awake monkey. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[87]  Sophia Rabe-Hesketh,et al.  A Nondegenerate Penalized Likelihood Estimator for Variance Parameters in Multilevel Models , 2013, Psychometrika.