Organizing and activating effects of sex hormones in homosexual transsexuals.

The cause of transsexualism remains unclear. The hypothesis that atypical prenatal hormone exposure could be a factor in the development of transsexualism was examined by establishing whether an atypical pattern of cognitive functioning was present in homosexual transsexuals. Possible activating effects of sex hormones as a result of cross-sex hormone treatment were also studied. Female-to-male and male-to-female transsexuals were compared with female and male controls with respect to spatial ability before and after treatment. The data were consistent with an organizing effect, but there was no evidence of an activating effect. Homosexual transsexuals, who prior to hormone treatment scored in the direction of the opposite sex, may have reached a ceiling in performance and therefore do not benefit from activating hormonal effects.

[1]  D. Kimura,et al.  Intrapersonal motor but not extrapersonal targeting skill is enhanced during the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle , 1998 .

[2]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[3]  M. Hassler Anomalous dominance, immune parameters, and spatial ability. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.

[4]  S. Resnick,et al.  Early hormonal influences on cognitive functioning in congenital adrenal hyperplasia. , 1986 .

[5]  D. Kimura,et al.  Reciprocal effects of hormonal fluctuations on human motor and perceptual-spatial skills. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  S. F. Witelson,et al.  A cognitive profile of homosexual men compared to heterosexual men and women , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[7]  J. Bailey,et al.  Sexual Orientation of Female-to-Male Transsexuals: A Comparison of Homosexual and Nonhomosexual Types , 2000, Archives of sexual behavior.

[8]  Irwin Silverman,et al.  Effects of estrogen changes during the menstrual cycle on spatial performance , 1993 .

[9]  Cognitive Style, Hemispheric Specialization, and Tested Abilities of Transsexuals and Nontranssexuals , 1976, Perceptual and motor skills.

[10]  R. Blanchard Typology of male-to-female transsexualism , 1985, Archives of sexual behavior.

[11]  F. Leavitt,et al.  Clinical patterns among male transsexual candidates with erotic interest in males , 1990, Archives of sexual behavior.

[12]  A. Bartfai,et al.  General intelligence and cognitive profile in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) , 1994, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[13]  J. Bailey,et al.  Spatial ability, handedness, and human sexual orientation , 1995, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  S. Stone,et al.  Gonadal hormones and cognitive performance , 1978 .

[15]  R. Blanchard,et al.  Prediction of Regrets in Postoperative Transsexuals , 1989, Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie.

[16]  Onur Güntürkün,et al.  Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. , 2000 .

[17]  Elizabeth Hampson,et al.  Cognitive Pattern in Men and Women Is Influenced by Fluctuations in Sex Hormones , 1994 .

[18]  A. Verschoor,et al.  Cross-gender identity in transvestites and male transsexuals , 1994, Archives of sexual behavior.

[19]  Susan D. Voyer,et al.  Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  S. H. Goozen,et al.  COGNITIVE ABILITY AND CEREBRAL LATERALISATION IN TRANSSEXUALS , 1998, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[21]  R. Pillard,et al.  Sexual orientation and cognitive abilities , 1991, Archives of sexual behavior.

[22]  J. Gilger,et al.  Effects of Menstrual Cycle on Spatial Information-Processes , 1986, Perceptual and motor skills.

[23]  Maria I. New,et al.  Gender Change from Female to Male in Classical Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia , 1996, Hormones and Behavior.

[24]  S. H. Goozen,et al.  Activating effects of cross-sex hormones on cognitive functioning: a study of short-term and long-term hormone effects in transsexuals , 1999, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[25]  A. Benton Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual , 1983 .

[26]  D. Kimura,et al.  Sexual orientation and performance on sexually dimorphic motor tasks , 1995, Archives of sexual behavior.

[27]  D. Wegesin,et al.  A Neuropsychologic Profile of Homosexual and Heterosexual Men and Women , 1998, Archives of sexual behavior.

[28]  H. W. Gordon,et al.  Changes in Specialized Cognitive Function Following Changes in Hormone Levels , 1986, Cortex.

[29]  Nico H. Frijda,et al.  Activating effects of androgens on cognitive performance: Causal evidence in a group of female-to-male transsexuals , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  D. Halpern Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities , 1986 .

[31]  F. Slijper,et al.  Long-Term Psychological Evaluation of Intersex Children , 1998, Archives of sexual behavior.

[32]  D. Hunt,et al.  The relationship of male transsexual typology to psychosocial adjustment , 1990, Archives of sexual behavior.

[33]  R. Blanchard,et al.  Gender dysphoria, gender reorientation, and the clinical management of transsexualism. , 1985, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[34]  D. Kimura,et al.  The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[35]  R. Deyo,et al.  Testosterone, dihydrotestosterone, and spatial task performances of males , 1990 .

[36]  Diane F. Halpern,et al.  Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities. Third Edition. , 2000 .

[37]  Scott D. Moffat,et al.  A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: Possible influence of hand preference , 1996, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[38]  R. Blanchard,et al.  Comparison of height and weight in homosexual versus nonhomosexual male gender dysphorics , 1995, Archives of sexual behavior.

[39]  R. Deyo,et al.  ANDROGENS AND SPATIAL ABILITY: FAILURE TO FIND A RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TESTOSTERONE AND ABILITY MEASURES , 1987 .

[40]  R. Blanchard Nonhomosexual gender dysphoria. , 1988, Journal of sex research.

[41]  N. Frijda,et al.  Gender differences in behaviour: Activating effects of cross-sex hormones , 1995, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[42]  K. Christiansen,et al.  Sex hormones and cognitive functioning in men. , 1987, Neuropsychobiology.

[43]  J. Greene Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment , 1995 .

[44]  Elizabeth Hampson,et al.  Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[45]  Van Strien,et al.  Classificatie van links- en rechtshandige proefpersonen. , 1992 .

[46]  H. W. Gordon,et al.  A relationship between gonadotropins and visuospatial function , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  R. Blanchard,et al.  H-Y antigen and homosexuality in men. , 1997, Journal of theoretical biology.

[48]  K. Christiansen Sex hormone-related variations of cognitive performance in !Kung San hunter-gatherers of Namibia. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[49]  L. Hubert,et al.  The relationship between androgen levels and human spatial abilities , 1983 .

[50]  R. Blanchard,et al.  The classification and labeling of nonhomosexual gender dysphorias , 1989, Archives of sexual behavior.

[51]  P. Irwin Greater brain response of left-handers to drugs , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[53]  G. Sanders,et al.  Estrogen and Memory in a Transsexual Population , 1998, Hormones and Behavior.

[54]  N. Mcconaghy,et al.  Two Clinically Discrete Syndromes of Transsexualism , 1978, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[55]  N. Neave,et al.  Sex Differences in Cognition: The Role of Testosterone and Sexual Orientation , 1999, Brain and Cognition.