The haptic perception of spatial orientations

This review examines the isotropy of the perception of spatial orientations in the haptic system. It shows the existence of an oblique effect (i.e., a better perception of vertical and horizontal orientations than oblique orientations) in a spatial plane intrinsic to the haptic system, determined by the gravitational cues and the cognitive resources and defined in a subjective frame of reference. Similar results are observed from infancy to adulthood. In 3D space, the haptic processing of orientations is also anisotropic and seems to use both egocentric and allocentric cues. Taken together, these results revealed that the haptic oblique effect occurs when the sensory motor traces associated with exploratory movement are represented more abstractly at a cognitive level.

[1]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Geometrical haptic illusions: The role of exploration in the Müller-Lyer, vertical-horizontal, and Delboeuf illusions , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Distance-Location Interference in Movement Reproduction , 2000 .

[4]  M Lipshits,et al.  Gravity affects the preferred vertical and horizontal in visual perception of orientation. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[5]  L'inférence haptique d'une localisation spatiale chez les adultes et les enfants : etude de l'effet du trajet et du délai dans une tâche de « complètement de triangle » , 2006 .

[6]  Michel Guerraz,et al.  Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[8]  R. Held,et al.  Anisotropic resolution in children's vision , 1984, Vision Research.

[9]  W Gerbino,et al.  Retinal vs. environmental orientation in the perception of the right angle. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[10]  Eric L Groen,et al.  Perception of Self-Tilt in a True and Illusory Vertical Plane , 2002, Perception.

[11]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  James T. Walker Tactual field dependence , 1972 .

[13]  Claire Leconte,et al.  The plasticity of gravitational reference frame and the subjective vertical: Peripheral visual information affects the oblique effect , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[14]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  S. Millar Understanding and Representing Space: Theory and Evidence from Studies with Blind and Sighted Children , 1994 .

[16]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Body tilt effect on the reproduction of orientations: studies on the visual oblique effect and subjective orientations. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  G. Westheimer Meridional anisotropy in visual processing: implications for the neural site of the oblique effect , 2003, Vision Research.

[18]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  É. Gentaz Existe-t-il un « effet de l'oblique » dans la perception tactile des orientations? , 2000 .

[20]  Arlette Streri,et al.  Infants' Haptic Discrimination of Spatial Orientations , 2002 .

[21]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptic perception of parallelity in the midsagittal plane. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[22]  P. Viviani,et al.  Factors affecting the size of the detour effect in the kinaesthetic perception of Euclidean distance , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  J. Paillard Brain and space , 1991 .

[24]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[25]  Ning Qian,et al.  The oblique effect depends on perceived, rather than physical, orientation and direction , 2005, Vision Research.

[26]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  Extracting object properties through haptic exploration. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[27]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  S Appelle,et al.  Eliminating the Haptic Oblique Effect: Influence of Scanning Incongruity and Prior Knowledge of the Standards , 1986, Perception.

[29]  Anne R. Schutte,et al.  Toward a formal theory of flexible spatial behavior: geometric category biases generalize across pointing and verbal response types. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  M. Robert,et al.  Traité de psychologie expérimentale , 1994 .

[31]  W. R. Garner,et al.  Internal frame of reference as a determinant of the oblique effect. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Orientation anisotropy in vernier acuity , 1995, Vision Research.

[33]  M. Banks,et al.  The effect of head tilt on meridional differences in acuity: Implications for orientation constancy , 1975 .

[34]  Pâris EXISTE-T-IL UN « EFFET DE L'OBLIQUE » DANS LA PERCEPTION TACTILE DES ORIENTATIONS ? , 2000 .

[35]  J. Gibson Observations on active touch. , 1962, Psychological review.

[36]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptic Perception of Spatial Relations , 1999, Perception.

[37]  System der optischen und haptischen Raumtäuschungen. , 1934 .

[38]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Bias and sensitivity in the haptic perception of geometry , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Cécile Ballaz,et al.  La perception visuelle des orientations et« l'effet de l'oblique » , 2000 .

[40]  Christian Marendaz,et al.  Visual Search for Oriented Lines: Anisotropy and Psychophysical Modeling Nature and Dynamics of Reference Frames Nature and Dynamics of Reference Frames in Visual Search for Orientation: Implications for Early Visual Processing , 2022 .

[41]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptically Straight Lines , 2007, Perception.

[42]  Yvette Hatwell,et al.  Form Perception and Related Issues in Blind Humans , 1978 .

[43]  E. Gentaz,et al.  The haptic oblique effect in the perception of rod orientation by blind adults , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  E C Lechelt,et al.  Spatial Anisotropy in Intramodal and Cross-Modal Judgments of Stimulus Orientation: The Stability of the Oblique Effect , 1980, Perception.

[45]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Cognitive allocentric representations of visual space shape pointing errors , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  S Appelle,et al.  Effect of Modality-Specific Experience on Visual and Haptic Judgment of Orientation , 1985, Perception.

[47]  A. Streri,et al.  Touching for knowing : cognitive psychology of haptic manual perception , 2003 .

[48]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  The manual haptic perception of orientations and the oblique effect in patients with left visuo-spatial neglect , 2002, Neuroreport.

[49]  A. Columbus Advances in Psychology Research , 2005 .

[50]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Approaches to the study of haptic sensing. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  G. Allen,et al.  Bias in spatial memory: a categorical endorsement. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[52]  C. Furmanski,et al.  An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Haptic interaction with virtual objects , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[54]  John P Spencer,et al.  Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  Susanna Millar,et al.  Space and Sense , 2008 .

[56]  Richard Held,et al.  Infant visual acuity and its meridional variation , 1978, Vision Research.

[57]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science , 2003 .

[58]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  Philippe Kahane,et al.  Reappraisal of the human vestibular cortex by cortical electrical stimulation study , 2003, Annals of neurology.

[60]  Edward A. Essock,et al.  Superior sensitivity for tactile stimuli oriented proximally-distally on the finger : Implications for mixed class 1 and class 2 anisotropies , 1997 .

[61]  D. Regan,et al.  Periodicity in orientation discrimination and the unconfounding of visual information , 1986, Vision Research.

[62]  E. Essock The Oblique Effect of Stimulus Identification Considered with Respect to Two Classes of Oblique Effects , 1980, Perception.

[63]  B. Frost,et al.  Human Visual Ecology and Orientation Anisotropies in Acuity , 1973, Science.

[64]  J. Spencer,et al.  The Emerging Spatial Mind , 2007 .

[65]  L. Hedges,et al.  Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.

[66]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Beyond dissociation : interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing , 2000 .

[67]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  The role of contextual cues in the haptic perception of orientations and the oblique effect , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[68]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Early visual experience affects memorization and spatial representation of proprioceptive targets , 1996, Neuroreport.

[69]  W. D. Walsh,et al.  Memory for constrained and preselected movement location and distance. , 1979, Journal of motor behavior.

[70]  The influence of body position on perception of orientation in infants , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[71]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  The Reproduction of Vertical and Oblique Orientations in the Visual, Haptic, and Somatovestibular Systems , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[72]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Interview: Mark D'Esposito with Michael S. Gazzaniga , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  J. S. Wright,et al.  The oblique effect in orientation acuity , 1997, Vision Research.

[74]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Large Systematic Deviations in the Haptic Perception of Parallelity , 1999, Perception.

[75]  Susanna Millar Understanding and representing space , 1994 .

[76]  François Dupuy,et al.  The Frame of Reference , 1999 .

[77]  R Held,et al.  Orientational anisotropy in infant vision , 1975, Science.

[78]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  The contributions of egocentric and allocentric reference frames in haptic spatial tasks. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[79]  E. Gentaz,et al.  The Haptic ‘Oblique Effect’ in Children's and Adults' Perception of Orientation , 1995, Perception.

[80]  S Westland,et al.  Multiple groups of orientation–selective visual mechanisms underlying rapid orientated–line detection , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[81]  E. Lechelt,et al.  Perceptual orientational asymmetries: A comparison of visual and haptic space , 1976 .

[82]  D. Levi,et al.  Meridional Anisotropy in the Discrimination of Parallel and Perpendicular Lines—Effect of Body Tilt , 1996, Perception.

[83]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  What does theoretical neuroscience have to offer the study of behavioral development?:Insights from a dynamic field theory of spatial cognition , 2007 .

[84]  Orientation anisotropy: some caveats in interpreting group differences and developmental changes , 1992, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[85]  R. Freeman,et al.  Oblique effect: a neural basis in the visual cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[86]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[87]  K. Sathian Touch, Representation, and Blindness. , 2001 .

[88]  P Viviani,et al.  Pointing to Kinesthetic Targets in Space , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[89]  M. Mcmahon,et al.  The origin of the oblique effect examined with pattern adaptation and masking. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[90]  Marc Green,et al.  The oblique effect in a vernier acuity situation , 1977 .

[91]  Susanna Millar,et al.  The Müller-Lyer illusion in touch and vision: Implications for multisensory processes , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[92]  H. Pashler,et al.  Improvement in line orientation discrimination is retinally local but dependent on cognitive set , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[93]  A. Streri,et al.  Reference frame and haptic discrimination of orientations in infants , 2005, Neuroreport.

[94]  G. Révész,et al.  Psychology and art of the blind , 1950 .

[95]  D W Heeley,et al.  Anisotropic Axes in Orientation Perception are Not Retinotopically Mapped , 1993, Perception.

[96]  Albert Postma,et al.  Delay improves performance on a haptic spatial matching task , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[97]  E Gentaz,et al.  Role of memorization conditions in the haptic processing of orientations and the 'oblique effect'. , 1999, British journal of psychology.

[98]  A. Streri,et al.  Factors Influencing Manual Discrimination of Orientations in 5-Month-Old Infants , 2009, Perception.

[99]  F. Attneave,et al.  Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation , 1967 .

[100]  Rüdiger Wenzel,et al.  Human Vestibular Cortex as Identified with Caloric Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, NeuroImage.

[101]  A. Streri,et al.  An Oblique Effect in Infants' Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[102]  Gabriel Baud-Bovy,et al.  The haptic reproduction of orientations in three-dimensional space , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[103]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Recognition of stimulus orientation , 1990, Vision Research.

[104]  Guy Cheron,et al.  Two Reference Frames for Visual Perception in Two Gravity Conditions , 2005, Perception.