Calibration of metacognitive judgments: Insights from the underconfidence-with-practice effect

Research on calibration typically compares mean judgments-of-learning (JOLs) made on a 0–100% scale with mean recall to determine whether there is overconfidence (JOL > recall), underconfidence (JOL < recall), or realism (JOL = recall). This research is founded on the assumption that JOLs directly reflect underlying representations of probability, which, if true, should mean that results from studies employing per cent JOL scales will generalize to other measures of subjective probability. We report four experiments that tested this assumption in the context of the underconfidence-with-practice (UWP) effect, the finding that JOLs underestimate recall on second and subsequent study-test cycles of multi-cycle paired-associate learning tasks (e.g., Koriat, Sheffer, & Ma’ayan, 2002). In particular, after replicating the standard UWP effect with scale JOLs in Experiment 1, we tested whether the effect would generalize to a binary JOL judgment in Experiment 2 and/or a binary betting decision in Experiment 3. In neither experiment was the UWP effect observed. Finally, to ensure that memorial and metamemorial evidence was equated for each judgment type, each item was assigned both a scale JOL and a binary betting decision in Experiment 4. UWP was observed for the 0–100% scale JOL, but not the binary betting decision. Together, the results suggest that JOLs may not directly reflect subjective probability but rather reflect an initial yes/no decision about future recall followed by a confidence rating.

[1]  Bridgid Finn,et al.  Framing effects on metacognitive monitoring and control , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[2]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .

[3]  Anders Winman,et al.  Calibration and diagnosticity of confidence in eyewitness identification: Comments on what can be inferred from the low confidence-accuracy correlation , 1996 .

[4]  D. Calvillo Working memory and the memory distortion component of hindsight bias , 2012, Memory.

[5]  Philip A. Higham,et al.  Regulation of memory accuracy with multiple answers: the plurality option. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[6]  Janet Metcalfe,et al.  Judgments of Learning are Influenced by Memory for Past Test. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[7]  Katherine A. Rawson,et al.  Second-Order Judgments About Judgments of Learning , 2005 .

[8]  Alon Y. Halevy,et al.  Semantic Integration , 2005, AI Mag..

[9]  A. Koriat,et al.  Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Jeffrey D. Karpicke,et al.  Correcting a metacognitive error: feedback increases retention of low-confidence correct responses. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  Janet Metcalfe,et al.  Metacognition and control of study choice in children , 2013 .

[12]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Hindsight ≠ foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty* , 2003 .

[13]  P. Rast,et al.  Age Differences in the Underconfidence-With-Practice Effect , 2009, Experimental aging research.

[14]  Christopher N. Wahlheim,et al.  Predicting memory performance under conditions of proactive interference: Immediate and delayed judgments of learning , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[15]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Probabilistic mental models: a Brunswikian theory of confidence. , 1991, Psychological review.

[16]  N. Brewer,et al.  The Effect of Retention Interval on the Confidence–Accuracy Relationship for Eyewitness Identification , 2010, Law and human behavior.

[17]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[18]  P. Higham,et al.  Trace‐strength and source‐monitoring accounts of accuracy and metacognitive resolution in the misinformation paradigm , 2011 .

[19]  Carl Martin Allwood,et al.  The Cognitive Interview: Effects on the realism in witnesses’ confidence in their free recall , 2005 .

[20]  Gideon Keren,et al.  On The Calibration of Probability Judgments: Some Critical Comments and Alternative Perspectives , 1997 .

[21]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  Misinformation Effects in Recall: Creating False Memories through Repeated Retrieval , 1996 .

[22]  Gideon Keren,et al.  Why isp = .90 better thanp = .70? Preference for definitive predictions by lay consumers of probability judgments , 2001 .

[23]  P. Higham Believing details known to have been suggested , 1998 .

[24]  G. Keren Calibration and probability judgements: Conceptual and methodological issues , 1991 .

[25]  Michael J. Serra,et al.  Magnitude and accuracy differences between judgements of remembering and forgetting , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  E F Loftus,et al.  Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[27]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Lack of pervasiveness of the underconfidence-with-practice effect: boundary conditions and an explanation via anchoring. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .

[29]  A. Castel,et al.  Betting on memory leads to metacognitive improvement by younger and older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Comparing Gains and Losses , 2010, Psychological science.

[31]  Michael J. Serra,et al.  Does retrieval fluency contribute to the underconfidence-with-practice effect? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[32]  Daniel Mochon,et al.  A scale distortion theory of anchoring. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  Neil Brewer,et al.  The confidence-accuracy relationship in eyewitness identification: effects of lineup instructions, foil similarity, and target-absent base rates. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[34]  Janet Metcalfe,et al.  The role of memory for past test in the underconfidence with practice effect. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Michael J. Serra,et al.  The contributions of anchoring and past-test performance to the underconfidence-with-practice effect , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[36]  Barbara A. Spellman,et al.  When Predictions Create Reality: Judgments of Learning May Alter What They Are Intended to Assess , 1992 .

[37]  Mats Björkman,et al.  Brunswikian and Thurstonian Origins of Bias in Probability Assessment: On the Interpretation of Stochastic Components of Judgment , 1997 .

[38]  A. Koriat Monitoring one's own knowledge during study : A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning , 1997 .

[39]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Hindsight is not equal to foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. , 1975 .

[40]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy , 1996 .

[41]  J. Dunlosky,et al.  Age-related differences in absolute but not relative metamemory accuracy. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[42]  Caren M Rotello,et al.  Sources of bias in the Goodman-Kruskal gamma coefficient measure of association: implications for studies of metacognitive processes. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  Lili Sahakyan,et al.  Memory, monitoring, and control in the attainment of memory accuracy , 2003 .

[44]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Predicting one's own forgetting: the role of experience-based and theory-based processes. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[46]  J. Dunlosky,et al.  The sensitivity of judgment-of-learning resolution to past test performance, new learning, and forgetting , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[47]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  I knew it would happen: Remembered probabilities of once—future things , 1975 .

[48]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Exploring a mnemonic debiasing account of the underconfidence-with-practice effect. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.