Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory

Seven experiments examined the spatial reference systems used in memory to represent the locations of objects in the environment. Participants learned the locations of common objects in a room and then made judgments of relative direction using their memories of the layout (e.g., "Imagine you are standing at the shoe, facing the lamp; point to the clock"). The experiments manipulated the number of views that observers were allowed to experience, the presence or absence of local and global reference systems (e.g., a rectangular mat on which objects were placed and the walls of the room, respectively), and the congruence of local and global reference systems. Judgments of relative direction were more accurate for imagined headings parallel to study views than for imagined headings parallel to novel views, even with up to three study views. However, study views misaligned with salient reference systems in the environment were not strongly represented if they were experienced in the context of aligned views. Novel views aligned with a local reference system were, under certain conditions, easier to imagine than were novel views misaligned with the local reference system. We propose that learning and remembering the spatial structure of the surrounding environment involves interpreting the layout in terms of a spatial reference system. This reference system is imposed on the environment but defined by egocentric experience.

[1]  S. Edelman,et al.  Canonical views in object representation and recognition , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  A. Wolters,et al.  Dynamics in Psychology , 1943, Nature.

[4]  J R Pani,et al.  Spatial Reference Systems in the Comprehension of Rotational Motion , 1994, Perception.

[5]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  H. Pick,et al.  Spatial orientation : theory, research, and application , 1984 .

[7]  T. Poggio,et al.  The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition , 1994, Current Biology.

[8]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  The spatial frame of reference in object naming and discrimination of left-right reflections , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Comment on Asch and Witkin's "Studies in space orientation: II." , 1992 .

[10]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[11]  B. Tversky Distortions in memory for maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  T. McNamara,et al.  Mental representations of large and small spatial layouts are orientation dependent. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Erich Goldmeier,et al.  Über Ähnlichkeit bei gesehenen Figuren , 1937 .

[14]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Some Demonstrations of the Effects of Structural Descriptions in Mental Imagery , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior. , 1980 .

[17]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[18]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation Specificity in Representations of Place , 1997 .

[19]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Orientation in cognitive maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  Stephen E. Palmer,et al.  Reference frames in the perception of shape and orientation , 1989 .

[21]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? , 1993, Cognition.

[22]  F. Attneave,et al.  Voluntary control of frame of reference and slope equivalence under head rotation. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  I. Rock,et al.  The effect of retinal and phenomenal orientation on the perception of form. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.

[24]  G. Logan Linguistic and Conceptual Control of Visual Spatial Attention , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[26]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[27]  Gordon T. Shippey,et al.  Determinants of the Perception of Rotational Motion : Orientation of the Motion to the Object and to the Environment , 1995 .

[28]  M. Tarr,et al.  Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  L. Acredolo,et al.  Laboratory versus home: The effect of environment on the 9-month-old infant's choice of spatial reference system. , 1979 .

[30]  S. Levinson,et al.  LANGUAGE AND SPACE , 1996 .

[31]  S. Levinson Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Cross-linguistic evidence , 1996 .

[32]  T. McNamara,et al.  Multiple views of spatial memory , 1997 .

[33]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation-specificity in kinesthetic spatial learning: The role of multiple orientations , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[34]  L. Acredolo Development of Spatial Orientation in Infancy. , 1978 .

[35]  A. Friedman,et al.  The importance of being upright: Use of environmental and viewer-centered reference frames in shape discriminations of novel three-dimensional objects , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[36]  T. McNamara,et al.  Viewpoint Dependence in Scene Recognition , 1997 .

[37]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  How Language Structures Space , 1983 .

[38]  Refractor Vision , 2000, The Lancet.

[39]  J. Rieser,et al.  Sensitivity to Perspective Structure While Walking without Vision , 1986, Perception.

[40]  I. Rock The orientation of forms on the retina and in the environment. , 1956, The American journal of psychology.