Inhibiting Food Reward: Delay Discounting, Food Reward Sensitivity, and Palatable Food Intake in Overweight and Obese Women

Overeating is believed to result when the appetitive motivation to consume palatable food exceeds an individual's capacity for inhibitory control of eating. This hypothesis was supported in recent studies involving predominantly normal weight women, but has not been tested in obese populations. The current study tested the interaction between food reward sensitivity and inhibitory control in predicting palatable food intake among energy‐replete overweight and obese women (N = 62). Sensitivity to palatable food reward was measured with the Power of Food Scale. Inhibitory control was assessed with a computerized choice task that captures the tendency to discount large delayed rewards relative to smaller immediate rewards. Participants completed an eating in the absence of hunger protocol in which homeostatic energy needs were eliminated with a bland preload of plain oatmeal, followed by a bogus laboratory taste test of palatable and bland snacks. The interaction between food reward sensitivity and inhibitory control was a significant predictor of palatable food intake in regression analyses controlling for BMI and the amount of preload consumed. Probing this interaction indicated that higher food reward sensitivity predicted greater palatable food intake at low levels of inhibitory control, but was not associated with intake at high levels of inhibitory control. As expected, no associations were found in a similar regression analysis predicting intake of bland foods. Findings support a neurobehavioral model of eating behavior in which sensitivity to palatable food reward drives overeating only when accompanied by insufficient inhibitory control. Strengthening inhibitory control could enhance weight management programs.

[1]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[2]  P. Muris,et al.  Individual differences in reward sensitivity are related to food craving and relative body weight in healthy women , 2005, Appetite.

[3]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[4]  L. Green,et al.  Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  E. Reiman,et al.  Successful dieters have increased neural activity in cortical areas involved in the control of behavior , 2007, International Journal of Obesity.

[6]  C. Davis,et al.  Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity , 2010, Appetite.

[7]  B. Reynolds A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[8]  R. Guerrieri,et al.  The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket , 2009, International Journal of Obesity.

[9]  J. Sinacore Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions , 1993 .

[10]  Daniel D. Holt,et al.  Discounting of Monetary and Directly Consumable Rewards , 2007, Psychological science.

[11]  Anne Roefs,et al.  Control yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods. , 2010, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[12]  J. Michael Schurr,et al.  Relation Between Obesity and Blunted Striatal Response to Food Is Moderated by Taq IA A 1 Allele , 2022 .

[13]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Caroline Reid,et al.  From motivation to behaviour: A model of reward sensitivity, overeating, and food preferences in the risk profile for obesity , 2007, Appetite.

[15]  W. Bickel,et al.  Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[16]  H. Berthoud,et al.  Appetite control and energy balance regulation in the modern world: reward-driven brain overrides repletion signals , 2009, International Journal of Obesity.

[17]  E. Reiman,et al.  Less activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in response to a meal: a feature of obesity. , 2006, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[18]  B. Appelhans,et al.  Neurobehavioral Inhibition of Reward‐driven Feeding: Implications for Dieting and Obesity , 2009, Obesity.

[19]  Christopher N. Ochner,et al.  The Power of Food Scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment , 2009, Appetite.

[20]  A. Odum,et al.  Discounting of delayed hypothetical money and food: Effects of amount , 2006, Behavioural Processes.

[21]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[22]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  The right brain hypothesis for obesity. , 2007, JAMA.

[23]  A. Bechara Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Fulton Appetite and reward , 2010, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology.

[25]  Jean Logan,et al.  Brain dopamine and obesity , 2001, The Lancet.

[26]  Christina M. Legierski,et al.  Overweight children find food more reinforcing and consume more energy than do nonoverweight children. , 2008, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[27]  L. Epstein,et al.  Food reinforcement, the dopamine D2 receptor genotype, and energy intake in obese and nonobese humans. , 2007, Behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  M. Commons The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value , 2013 .

[29]  K. Berridge,et al.  The tempted brain eats: Pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders , 2010, Brain Research.

[30]  A. Hariri,et al.  Preference for Immediate over Delayed Rewards Is Associated with Magnitude of Ventral Striatal Activity , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Leonard H. Epstein,et al.  Food reinforcement, delay discounting and obesity , 2010, Physiology & Behavior.

[32]  L. Epstein,et al.  Reinforcing Value of Food in Obese and Non-obese Women , 1996, Appetite.

[33]  M. Butryn,et al.  Hedonic hunger: A new dimension of appetite? , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  D. Ridder,et al.  Evolved to satisfy our immediate needs: Self-control and the rewarding properties of food , 2006, Appetite.

[35]  Eric Stice,et al.  Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: An fMRI study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  R. Gerber,et al.  Evaluating the Power of Food Scale in obese subjects and a general sample of individuals: development and measurement properties , 2009, International Journal of Obesity.

[37]  R. Weller,et al.  Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women , 2008, Appetite.

[38]  Leonard H. Epstein,et al.  Delay discounting moderates the effect of food reinforcement on energy intake among non-obese women , 2010, Appetite.

[39]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[40]  Saori C. Tanaka,et al.  Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Kewei Chen,et al.  Less activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the reanalysis of the response to a meal in obese than in lean women and its association with successful weight loss. , 2007, The American journal of clinical nutrition.