Olfactory associative learning in Caenorhabditis elegans is impaired in lrn-1 and lrn-2 mutants.

The C. elegans mutants, lrn-1 and lrn-2, are impaired in associative learning using conditioned taste cues. Both mutants are defective in associative learning about appetitive and aversive events, indicating that lrn-1 and lrn-2 exert effects across motivational boundaries. In a new olfactory associative learning paradigm, in which wild type worms learn to avoid a previously attractive diacetyl odor after it has been paired with an aversive acetic acid solution, lrn-1 and lrn-2 are impaired. Although defective in associative learning using a conditioned olfactory cue, nonassociative learning (habituation and dishabituation) using this same olfactory cue is unaffected. The discovery that lrn-1 and lrn-2 are defective in associative learning with both taste and olfactory cues may suggest that associative learning in different sensory modalities converges on a common genetic pathway in C. elegans that is subserved by lrn-1 and lrn-2.

[1]  C. Rankin,et al.  Long-term habituation is produced by distributed training at long ISIs and not by massed training or short ISIs inCaenorhabditis elegans , 1997 .

[2]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  OSM-9, A Novel Protein with Structural Similarity to Channels, Is Required for Olfaction, Mechanosensation, and Olfactory Adaptation inCaenorhabditis elegans , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  G. Ruvkun,et al.  Regulation of Interneuron Function in the C. elegans Thermoregulatory Pathway by the ttx-3 LIM Homeobox Gene , 1997, Neuron.

[4]  J. Grau,et al.  Mechanisms of Pavlovian conditioning: role of protection from habituation in spinal conditioning. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  odr-10 Encodes a Seven Transmembrane Domain Olfactory Receptor Required for Responses to the Odorant Diacetyl , 1996, Cell.

[6]  T. Tully,et al.  latheo, a new gene involved in associative learning and memory in Drosophila melanogaster, identified from P element mutagenesis. , 1992, Genetics.

[7]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  Chemosensory neurons with overlapping functions direct chemotaxis to multiple chemicals in C. elegans , 1991, Neuron.

[8]  C. H. Rankin,et al.  Caenorhabditis elegans: A new model system for the study of learning and memory , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  B. Schreurs Classical conditioning of model systems: A behavioral review , 1989, Psychobiology.

[10]  J. Culotti,et al.  Osmotic avoidance defective mutants of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. , 1978, Genetics.

[11]  L. Byerly,et al.  The life cycle of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. I. Wild-type growth and reproduction. , 1976, Developmental biology.

[12]  D B Dusenbery,et al.  Analysis of chemotaxis in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans by countercurrent separation. , 1974, The Journal of experimental zoology.

[13]  Cori Bargmann Genetic and cellular analysis of behavior in C. elegans. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  W. Wood The Nematode Caenorhabditis elegans , 1988 .

[15]  R. Rescorla Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. , 1967, Psychological review.