Vigilance, boredom proneness and detection time of a malfunction in partially automated driving

A vehicle automation makes it possible to hand over the control task from the driver to an automation, which is in turn monitored by the driver. In this study, it was investigated how long it takes for drivers to detect an automation malfunction and if the detection time can be predicted by performance in a self-developed vigilance task and by individual boredom proneness. 23 participants drove with partial automation (Level 2; [1, 2]) activated for 24 min on a three-lane highway. Dependent measure was the time until they detected a suddenly occurring malfunction of lateral control. No significant relationship between the predictors and detection time was found, which could be caused by the chosen operationalization of the dependent variable.

[1]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Ryan C. Martin,et al.  Driving anger, sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[3]  M. Scerbo What's so boring about vigilance? , 1998 .

[4]  B. Oken,et al.  Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  Carryl L. Baldwin,et al.  Driver fatigue: The importance of identifying causal factors of fatigue when considering detection and countermeasure technologies , 2009 .

[6]  J. R. Binford,et al.  Examination of some factors influencing performance on an auditory monitoring task with one signal per session. , 1970, Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[7]  Niklas Strand,et al.  Semi-automated versus highly automated driving in critical situations caused by automation failures , 2014 .

[8]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Individual differences in vigilance: Personality, ability and states of stress , 2010 .

[9]  K. Bengler,et al.  Interaction Principles for Cooperative Human-Machine Systems , 2012, it Inf. Technol..

[10]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Monitoring an Automated System for a Single Failure: Vigilance and Task Complexity Effects , 1996 .

[11]  I. Singh,et al.  Individual differences in monitoring failures of automation , 1993 .

[12]  A. Buchner,et al.  Drivers' misjudgement of vigilance state during prolonged monotonous daytime driving. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[13]  David B. Kaber,et al.  Situation awareness and workload in driving while using adaptive cruise control and a cell phone , 2005 .

[14]  W. Helton,et al.  Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[15]  Neville A. Stanton,et al.  Effects of adaptive cruise control and highly automated driving on workload and situation awareness: A review of the empirical evidence , 2014 .

[16]  Lisanne Bainbridge,et al.  Ironies of automation , 1982, Autom..

[17]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  The Effects of Signal Salience and Caffeine on Performance, Workload, and Stress in an Abbreviated Vigilance Task , 2000, Hum. Factors.

[18]  Klaus Bengler,et al.  Potential Individual Differences Regarding Automation Effects in Automated Driving , 2014, Interacción '14.

[19]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Automation- Induced "Complacency": Development of the Complacency-Potential Rating Scale , 1993 .

[20]  N. Mackworth The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .

[21]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Performance Consequences of Automation-Induced 'Complacency' , 1993 .

[22]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Predicting vigilance: A fresh look at an old problem , 2009, Ergonomics.

[23]  Mark S. Young,et al.  Cooperation between drivers and automation: implications for safety , 2009 .

[24]  Anthony N. Pettitt,et al.  Predicting driver's hypovigilance on monotonous roads: literature review. , 2009 .

[25]  Klaus Bengler,et al.  Trust in Automation – Before and After the Experience of Take-over Scenarios in a Highly Automated Vehicle☆ , 2015 .

[26]  R. Farmer,et al.  Boredom proneness--the development and correlates of a new scale. , 1986, Journal of personality assessment.

[27]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: linking mind wandering and behavioral variability. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[29]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  A Multivariate Test Battery for Predicting Vigilance , 2010 .

[30]  Moritz Körber,et al.  Prediction of take-over time in highly automated driving by two psychometric tests , 2015 .

[31]  William S. Helton,et al.  Visuospatial and verbal working memory load: effects on visuospatial vigilance , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Humans and Automation: Use, Misuse, Disuse, Abuse , 1997, Hum. Factors.

[33]  Thomas M. Spalek,et al.  Driving With the Wandering Mind , 2014, Hum. Factors.

[34]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  The role of memory representation in the vigilance decrement , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  Jacques Bergeron,et al.  Monotony of road environment and driver fatigue: a simulator study. , 2003, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[36]  Klaus Bengler,et al.  Taking Over Control from Highly Automated Vehicles , 2014 .

[37]  H. Gr Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload; a cognitive-energetical framework. , 1997 .