Differential effects of practice on the executive processes used for truthful and deceptive responses: an event-related brain potential study.

Behavior and event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded while participants made truthful and deceptive responses about previously memorized words under three instructional conditions: consistent truthful, consistent deceptive, and random deceptive. To determine if practice affected the deception-related activity we reported previously [R. Johnson, Jr., J. Barnhardt, J. Zhu, The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity. Biol. Psychol., 64 (2003) 217-253; R. Johnson, Jr., J. Barnhardt, J. Zhu, The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding. Neuropsychologia, 42 (2004) 878-901], participants performed two blocks of 145 trials of each condition. In the consistent truthful condition, practice benefited performance as indicated by decreased reaction time (RT) and RT variability. In addition, practice increased P300 amplitude and decreased the amplitude of a medial frontal negativity (MFN), which is believed to index the use of response-monitoring processes. However, a different pattern of results obtained in the two deception conditions. Although practice decreased RTs by almost as much as in the consistent truthful condition, the extent to which deceptive response in both conditions were slower than those in the consistent truthful condition actually increased slightly. Hence, the component of RT reflecting processing of conflicting response information did not decrease. In accord with the RT results, MFN amplitudes in the consistent deceptive and random deceptive conditions were unaffected by practice, suggesting that the amount of executive processes required to make and/or monitor deceptive responses was undiminished by practice. Although P300 amplitude increased slightly in the consistent deceptive condition, there was no change in the random deceptive condition. Thus, a major finding here is that, unlike truthful responses, the conceptually driven response conflicts underlying deceptive responses appear to be as resistant to practice-induced changes as described previously for perceptually driven response conflicts.

[1]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[2]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[3]  R. Shor,et al.  Effect of practice on a Stroop-like spatial directions task , 1972 .

[4]  Ray Johnson For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .

[5]  A F Kramer,et al.  Error-related processing during a period of extended wakefulness. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[6]  H Garavan,et al.  A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.

[7]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory. Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  A. Turken,et al.  Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus–response compatibility on brain activation , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[11]  G. Mangun,et al.  Effects of practice on executive control investigated with fMRI. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[13]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[14]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  A. Turken,et al.  Response selection in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  W. Gehring,et al.  Action-Monitoring Dysfunction in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2000, Psychological science.

[17]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  M. Coles,et al.  Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[20]  Jelena Jovanovic,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  E. Crone,et al.  Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  A. Saykin,et al.  Functional differentiation of medial temporal and frontal regions involved in processing novel and familiar words: an fMRI study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  S. Monsell Task-set reconfiguration processes do not imply a control homunuculus: Reply to Altmann , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  D. Tucker,et al.  Regulating action: alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  R. Johnson,et al.  A spatio-temporal analysis of recognition-related event-related brain potentials. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[28]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300: evidence from temporal lobectomy patients. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[29]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[30]  T. Shallice,et al.  Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[32]  D. Tucker,et al.  Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[34]  N. Yeung,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002 .

[35]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[36]  R. Johnson,et al.  On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[37]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[38]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  H. Lüders,et al.  American Electroencephalographic Society Guidelines for Standard Electrode Position Nomenclature , 1991, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[40]  D Friedman,et al.  Event‐related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.

[41]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[42]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[43]  J. Allman,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[44]  J. Knott,et al.  Regarding the American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature: a commentary on the proposal to change the 10-20 electrode designators. , 1993, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[45]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[46]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  P300 and long-term memory: latency predicts recognition performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[47]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[48]  F. Vidal,et al.  Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[49]  Ray Johnson,et al.  The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[50]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[51]  Robert W. Mitchell,et al.  Deception, perspectives on human and nonhuman deceit , 1986 .

[52]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[53]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  J. Sampson selection , 2006, Algorithm Design with Haskell.

[55]  L Nyberg,et al.  Brain activation during episodic memory retrieval: sex differences. , 2000, Acta psychologica.

[56]  John C Gore,et al.  An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[57]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[59]  E. Donchin,et al.  P300 and tracking difficulty: evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[60]  J J Furedy,et al.  Differentiation of deception as a psychological process: a psychophysiological approach. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[61]  Judith M Ford,et al.  Response-monitoring dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: an event-related potential study , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[62]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[63]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate and the Monitoring of Response Conflict: Evidence from an fMRI Study of Overt Verb Generation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  R. Johnson A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[65]  M Herrmann,et al.  Control of semantic interference in episodic memory retrieval is associated with an anterior cingulate‐prefrontal activation pattern , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[66]  W. Rogers,et al.  Mechanisms underlying reduction in Stroop interference with practice for young and old adults. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[67]  C D Wickens,et al.  The Event-Related Brain Potential as an Index of Display-Monitoring Workload , 1980, Human factors.

[68]  David Badre,et al.  Selection, Integration, and Conflict Monitoring Assessing the Nature and Generality of Prefrontal Cognitive Control Mechanisms , 2004, Neuron.

[69]  Andrew Webb,et al.  Behavioral conflict, anterior cingulate cortex, and experiment duration: Implications of diverging data , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[70]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[71]  S. Petersen,et al.  The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[72]  Ray Johnson,et al.  The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding , 2004, Neuropsychologia.