Differential effects of practice on the executive processes used for truthful and deceptive responses: an event-related brain potential study.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[2] John B. Carroll,et al. The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .
[3] R. Shor,et al. Effect of practice on a Stroop-like spatial directions task , 1972 .
[4] Ray Johnson. For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .
[5] A F Kramer,et al. Error-related processing during a period of extended wakefulness. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[6] H Garavan,et al. A midline dissociation between error-processing and response-conflict monitoring , 2003, NeuroImage.
[7] M. Botvinick,et al. Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[8] J. Desmond,et al. Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory. Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[9] A. Turken,et al. Dissociation between conflict detection and error monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[10] M. D’Esposito,et al. Neural implementation of response selection in humans as revealed by localized effects of stimulus–response compatibility on brain activation , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[11] G. Mangun,et al. Effects of practice on executive control investigated with fMRI. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[12] Adrian R. Willoughby,et al. The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.
[13] M. Posner,et al. Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .
[14] S. Monsell. Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] A. Turken,et al. Response selection in the human anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[16] W. Gehring,et al. Action-Monitoring Dysfunction in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 2000, Psychological science.
[17] H. Mayberg,et al. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[18] M. Coles,et al. Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[20] Jelena Jovanovic,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[21] E. Crone,et al. Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] A. Saykin,et al. Functional differentiation of medial temporal and frontal regions involved in processing novel and familiar words: an fMRI study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[23] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Cortex, Conflict Monitoring, and Levels of Processing , 2001, NeuroImage.
[24] S. Monsell. Task-set reconfiguration processes do not imply a control homunuculus: Reply to Altmann , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] D. Tucker,et al. Regulating action: alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[26] Colin M. Macleod,et al. Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[27] R. Johnson,et al. A spatio-temporal analysis of recognition-related event-related brain potentials. , 1998, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[28] R. Johnson,et al. On the neural generators of the P300: evidence from temporal lobectomy patients. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[29] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[30] T. Shallice,et al. Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[31] J. Cohen,et al. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.
[32] D. Tucker,et al. Medial Frontal Cortex in Action Monitoring , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[34] N. Yeung,et al. Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002 .
[35] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Event-related potential correlates of errors in reaction tasks. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[36] R. Johnson,et al. On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[37] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[38] R. Knight,et al. Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[39] H. Lüders,et al. American Electroencephalographic Society Guidelines for Standard Electrode Position Nomenclature , 1991, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[40] D Friedman,et al. Event‐related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review , 2000, Microscopy research and technique.
[41] J. Hohnsbein,et al. ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[42] M. Botvinick,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.
[43] J. Allman,et al. The Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[44] J. Knott,et al. Regarding the American Electroencephalographic Society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature: a commentary on the proposal to change the 10-20 electrode designators. , 1993, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[45] Clay B. Holroyd,et al. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.
[46] A Pfefferbaum,et al. P300 and long-term memory: latency predicts recognition performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.
[47] S. Rauch,et al. The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.
[48] F. Vidal,et al. Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[49] Ray Johnson,et al. The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[50] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[51] Robert W. Mitchell,et al. Deception, perspectives on human and nonhuman deceit , 1986 .
[52] K. Kiehl,et al. Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[53] C. Braun,et al. Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[54] J. Sampson. selection , 2006, Algorithm Design with Haskell.
[55] L Nyberg,et al. Brain activation during episodic memory retrieval: sex differences. , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[56] John C Gore,et al. An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[57] M. Posner,et al. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[58] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.
[59] E. Donchin,et al. P300 and tracking difficulty: evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[60] J J Furedy,et al. Differentiation of deception as a psychological process: a psychophysiological approach. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[61] Judith M Ford,et al. Response-monitoring dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: an event-related potential study , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.
[62] D. V. Cramon,et al. Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.
[63] T. Braver,et al. Anterior Cingulate and the Monitoring of Response Conflict: Evidence from an fMRI Study of Overt Verb Generation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[64] R. Johnson. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[65] M Herrmann,et al. Control of semantic interference in episodic memory retrieval is associated with an anterior cingulate‐prefrontal activation pattern , 2001, Human brain mapping.
[66] W. Rogers,et al. Mechanisms underlying reduction in Stroop interference with practice for young and old adults. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[67] C D Wickens,et al. The Event-Related Brain Potential as an Index of Display-Monitoring Workload , 1980, Human factors.
[68] David Badre,et al. Selection, Integration, and Conflict Monitoring Assessing the Nature and Generality of Prefrontal Cognitive Control Mechanisms , 2004, Neuron.
[69] Andrew Webb,et al. Behavioral conflict, anterior cingulate cortex, and experiment duration: Implications of diverging data , 2004, Human brain mapping.
[70] E. Donchin,et al. Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.
[71] S. Petersen,et al. The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[72] Ray Johnson,et al. The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding , 2004, Neuropsychologia.