Navigation in a Space With Moving Objects: Rats Can Avoid Specific Locations Defined With Respect to a Moving Robot

Animals can organize their behavior with respect to other moving animals or objects; when hunting or escaping a predator, when migrating in groups or during various social interactions. In rats, we aimed to characterize spatial behaviors relative to moving objects and to explore the cognitive mechanisms controlling these behaviors. Three groups of animals were trained to avoid a mild foot-shock delivered in one of three positions: either in front, on the left side, or on the right side of a moving robot. We showed the rats can recognize and avoid these specific areas. The avoidance behavior specific for the left or right side of the robot demonstrated animals not only react to “simple” stimuli such as increasing noise level or growing retinal image of an approaching object, but they process their spatial position relative to the object. Using an all-white robot without prominent visual patterns that would distinguish its different sides, we showed that the behavior does not depend on responses to prominent visual patterns, but that the rats can guide their navigation according to geometrical spatial relationship relative to the moving object. Rats’ competence for navigation in space defined by a moving object resembles navigation abilities in stationary space. Recording of hippocampal single unit activity during rat’s interaction with the robot proved feasibility of the task to uncover neuronal mechanism of this type of navigation.

[1]  K. Jeffery,et al.  Anisotropic encoding of three-dimensional space by place cells and grid cells , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  David C Rowland,et al.  Neural correlates of novel object and novel location recognition behavior in the mouse anterior cingulate cortex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. Muller,et al.  Representation of Objects in Space by Two Classes of Hippocampal Pyramidal Cells , 2004, The Journal of general physiology.

[4]  A. Stuchlik,et al.  Transient inactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex in rats disrupts avoidance of a dynamic object , 2017, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[5]  R. Muller,et al.  The firing of hippocampal place cells predicts the future position of freely moving rats , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  K. Jezek,et al.  Stress-Induced Out-of-Context Activation of Memory , 2010, PLoS biology.

[7]  R. Muller,et al.  The presence of a second rat has only subtle effects on the location‐specific firing of hippocampal place cells , 2012, Hippocampus.

[8]  Sang Geon Park,et al.  Alterations of Hippocampal Place Cells in Foraging Rats Facing a “Predatory” Threat , 2015, Current Biology.

[9]  Edvard I. Moser,et al.  Object-vector coding in the medial entorhinal cortex , 2018, bioRxiv.

[10]  A. Fenton,et al.  Dynamic Grouping of Hippocampal Neural Activity During Cognitive Control of Two Spatial Frames , 2010, PLoS biology.

[11]  Øyvind Arne Høydal,et al.  Object-vector coding in the medial entorhinal cortex , 2019, Nature.

[12]  Atsuo Takanishi,et al.  Modulation of rat behaviour by using a rat-like robot , 2013, Bioinspiration & biomimetics.

[13]  A. Treves,et al.  Theta-paced flickering between place-cell maps in the hippocampus , 2011, Nature.

[14]  Oliver T. Wolf,et al.  Stress and memory in humans: Twelve years of progress? , 2009, Brain Research.

[15]  Sachin S. Deshmukh,et al.  Egocentric coding of external items in the lateral entorhinal cortex , 2018, Science.

[16]  B. Roozendaal,et al.  Glucocorticoids and the regulation of memory in health and disease , 2009, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology.

[17]  R. Bolles,et al.  Clinical implications of Bolles & Fanselow's pain/fear model , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  David S. Touretzky,et al.  Place field dissociation and multiple maps in hippocampus , 2006, Neurocomputing.

[19]  S. A. Ho,et al.  Hippocampal place cell activity during chasing of a moving object associated with reward in rats , 2008, Neuroscience.

[20]  Jean-Marc Fellous,et al.  A new rat-compatible robotic framework for spatial navigation behavioral experiments , 2018, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[21]  K M Gothard,et al.  Binding of hippocampal CA1 neural activity to multiple reference frames in a landmark-based navigation task , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Joober,et al.  Declarative memory after stress in humans: differential involvement of the beta-adrenergic and corticosteroid systems. , 2005, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[23]  A. Fenton,et al.  Coordinating different representations in the hippocampus , 2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[24]  A. Fenton,et al.  Key Features of Human Episodic Recollection in the Cross-Episode Retrieval of Rat Hippocampus Representations of Space , 2013, PLoS biology.

[25]  J Bures,et al.  Operant behavior can be triggered by the position of the rat relative to objects rotating on an inaccessible platform , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  C. Barnes,et al.  The neural representation of 3-dimensional objects in rodent memory circuits , 2015, Behavioural Brain Research.

[27]  David C Rowland,et al.  Neural Correlates of Long-Term Object Memory in the Mouse Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  B. Roozendaal,et al.  Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  Sachin S. Deshmukh,et al.  Representation of Non-Spatial and Spatial Information in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex , 2011, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[30]  N. Ulanovsky,et al.  Social place-cells in the bat hippocampus , 2018, Science.

[31]  A. Redish Beyond the Cognitive Map: From Place Cells to Episodic Memory , 1999 .

[32]  W Wippich,et al.  Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. , 1996, Life sciences.

[33]  T. Toyoizumi,et al.  Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus , 2018, Science.

[34]  J. Born,et al.  Blocking Mineralocorticoid Receptors Impairs, Blocking Glucocorticoid Receptors Enhances Memory Retrieval in Humans , 2012, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[35]  E. Pastalkova,et al.  Enemy avoidance task: A novel behavioral paradigm for assessing spatial avoidance of a moving subject , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[36]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  An Information-Theoretic Approach to Deciphering the Hippocampal Code , 1992, NIPS.

[37]  C A Barnes,et al.  Representation of three‐dimensional objects by the rat perirhinal cortex , 2012, Hippocampus.

[38]  J. Born,et al.  Hippocampal corticosterone impairs memory consolidation during sleep but improves consolidation in the wake state , 2014, Hippocampus.

[39]  Michael S. Fanselow,et al.  A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. , 1988 .

[40]  Jan Born,et al.  Opposite Effects of Cortisol on Consolidation of Temporal Sequence Memory during Waking and Sleep , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  David Eilam,et al.  Traveling Companions Add Complexity and Hinder Performance in the Spatial Behavior of Rats , 2016, PloS one.

[42]  Sachin S. Deshmukh,et al.  Perirhinal cortex represents nonspatial, but not spatial, information in rats foraging in the presence of objects: Comparison with lateral entorhinal cortex , 2012, Hippocampus.

[43]  Rusalky del Angel Ortiz,et al.  Social Interaction Test between a Rat and a Robot: A Pilot Study , 2016 .

[44]  Ales Stuchlik,et al.  Functional inactivation of the rat hippocampus disrupts avoidance of a moving object , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.