Dual-task interference during initial learning of a new motor task results from competition for the same brain areas

Cerebral patterns of activity elicited by dual-task performance throughout the learning of a complex bimanual coordination pattern were addressed. Subjects (N=12) were trained on the coordination pattern and scanned using fMRI at early (PRE) and late (POST) learning stages. During scanning, the coordination pattern was performed either as a single task or in concurrence with a simultaneous visual search task (i.e. dual task). Kinematics data revealed a significant performance improvement as a result of learning. In PRE-scanning, the dual-task condition induced deterioration of motor performance, relative to the single-task condition. Activity in lateral frontal and parietal regions involved in both visual search and motor coordination tasks (i.e. 'overlapping' regions) was reduced when the tasks were performed simultaneously. In POST-scanning, kinematics performance was equivalent under single- and dual-task conditions, suggesting automaticity of the coordination pattern. Furthermore, overlap between regions involved in visual search and motor tasks was reduced, and dual-task performance was no longer associated with reduction of frontal and parietal activity. Our results suggest that behavioral interference between a complex motor coordination task and a simple simultaneous visual search task occurs when both tasks recruit overlapping regions in the frontal and parietal cortices. This may provide a neural basis for dissipation of dual-task interference following extensive motor practice, which is a traditional behavioral marker of motor automaticity.

[1]  Yuhong Jiang,et al.  Resolving dual-task interference: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Isolation of a Central Bottleneck of Information Processing with Time-Resolved fMRI , 2006, Neuron.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention to Action: Specific Modulation of Corticocortical Interactions in Humans , 2001, NeuroImage.

[6]  M. Hallett,et al.  How self-initiated memorized movements become automatic: a functional MRI study. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Changes in brain activation during the acquisition of a new bimanual coordination task , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory , 1994, Nature.

[9]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Dissociable Neural Effects of Task Order Control and Task Set Maintenance during Dual-task Processing , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Mark Hallett,et al.  The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in implicit procedural learning , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Central mechanisms of motor skill learning , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Correlates of Motor Skill Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  T. Klingberg Concurrent performance of two working memory tasks: potential mechanisms of interference. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  K. Amunts,et al.  Neural correlates of dual task interference can be dissociated from those of divided attention: an fMRI study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Motor Control and Learning , 2006 .

[17]  R. Magill Motor learning and control : concepts and applications , 2004 .

[18]  Paul Van Hecke,et al.  Internal vs external generation of movements: differential neural pathways involved in bimanual coordination performed in the presence or absence of augmented visual feedback , 2003, NeuroImage.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses , 1999, NeuroImage.

[20]  O Bock,et al.  Evidence for processing stages in skill acquisition: a dual-task study. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[21]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[22]  P M Grasby,et al.  Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al.  Model-free characterization of brain functional networks for motor sequence learning using fMRI , 2008, NeuroImage.

[24]  S. Swinnen Intermanual coordination: From behavioural principles to neural-network interactions , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  P. Roland,et al.  Interference between two concurrent tasks is associated with activation of overlapping fields in the cortex. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  Tony Ro,et al.  Human MST But Not MT Responds to Tactile Stimulation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  A. Kozulin The Learning Process , 1995 .

[29]  J. Kelso,et al.  Attentional demands reflect learning‐induced alterations of bimanual coordination dynamics , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  Jean-Luc Anton,et al.  Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .

[31]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[32]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Spatial interference during bimanual coordination: Differential brain networks associated with control of movement amplitude and direction , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[33]  O. Gruber Effects of domain-specific interference on brain activation associated with verbal working memory task performance. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[35]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Parieto-premotor areas mediate directional interference during bimanual movements. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Arthur F. Gmitro,et al.  Cortical mechanisms for acquisition and performance of bimanual motor sequences , 2003, NeuroImage.

[37]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Uncoupling Cognitive Workload and Prefrontal Cortical Physiology: A PET rCBF Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[38]  K. Zilles,et al.  Structural divisions and functional fields in the human cerebral cortex 1 Published on the World Wide Web on 20 February 1998. 1 , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.

[39]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[40]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of interference in overlapping dual tasks: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[41]  Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al.  Attention to movement modulates activity in sensori-motor areas, including primary motor cortex , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Trouble with Cognitive Subtraction , 1996, NeuroImage.

[43]  J. Kelso,et al.  Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .

[44]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[45]  W. Singer,et al.  The constructive nature of vision: direct evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion imagery , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  Nicole Wenderoth,et al.  Changes in Brain Activation during the Acquisition of a Multifrequency Bimanual Coordination Task: From the Cognitive Stage to Advanced Levels of Automaticity , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Ranganath,et al.  Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Promotes Long-Term Memory Formation through Its Role in Working Memory Organization , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Does excessive memory load attenuate activation in the prefrontal cortex? Load-dependent processing in single and dual tasks: functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[49]  R. Passingham,et al.  Learning Arbitrary Visuomotor Associations: Temporal Dynamic of Brain Activity , 2001, NeuroImage.

[50]  G. Essick,et al.  Tactile motion activates the human middle temporal/V5 (MT/V5) complex , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[51]  Paige E. Scalf,et al.  Training-induced functional activation changes in dual-task processing: an FMRI study. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Neural Evidence for Representation-Specific Response Selection , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[54]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[55]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.