Sound localization in noise and sensitivity to spectral shape

Individual differences exist in sound localization performance even for normal-hearing listeners. Some of these differences might be related to acoustical differences in localization cues carried by the head related transfer functions (HRTF). Recent data suggest that individual differences in sound localization performance could also have a perceptual origin. The localization of an auditory target in the up/down and front/back dimensions requires the analysis of the spectral shape of the stimulus. In the present study, we investigated the role of an acoustic factor, the prominence of the spectral shape ("spectral strength") and the role of a perceptual factor, the listener's sensitivity to spectral shape, in individual differences observed in sound localization performance. Spectral strength was computed as the spectral distance between the magnitude spectrum of the HRTFs and a flat spectrum. Sensitivity to spectral shape was evaluated using spectral-modulation thresholds measured with a broadband (0.2-12.8 kHz) or high-frequency (4-16 kHz) carrier and for different spectral modulation frequencies (below 1 cycle/octave, between 1 and 2 cycles/octave, above 2 cycles/octave). Data obtained from 19 young normal-hearing listeners showed that low thresholds for spectral modulation frequency below 1 cycle/octave with a high-frequency carrier were associated with better sound localization performance. No correlation was found between sound localization performance and the spectral strength of the HRTFs. These results suggest that differences in perceptual ability, rather than acoustical differences, contribute to individual differences in sound localization performance in noise.

[1]  E. Shaw Transformation of sound pressure level from the free field to the eardrum in the horizontal plane. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  F. Wightman,et al.  Acoustic origins of individual differences in sound localization behavior , 1988 .

[3]  Douglas S. Brungart,et al.  Head‐related transfer function enhancement for improved vertical‐polar localization. , 2009 .

[4]  Mark A. Ericson,et al.  A pointing technique for rapidly collecting localization responses in auditory research , 1995 .

[5]  André van Schaik,et al.  Auditory spatial perception with sources overlapping in frequency and time , 2005 .

[6]  John C Middlebrooks,et al.  Vertical-plane sound localization probed with ripple-spectrum noise. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Jinyu Qian,et al.  The role of spectral modulation cues in virtual sound localization. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  Pavel Zahorik,et al.  Perceptual recalibration in human sound localization: learning to remediate front-back reversals. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  Timothy R. Anderson,et al.  Factors Affecting the Relative Salience of Sound Localization Cues , 2014 .

[11]  Sophie Savel,et al.  Auditory Efferents Facilitate Sound Localization in Noise in Humans , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Binaural weighting of pinna cues in human sound localization , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Andrew T. Sabin,et al.  Perceptual learning of auditory spectral modulation detection , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Belinda A Henry,et al.  Spectral peak resolution and speech recognition in quiet: normal hearing, hearing impaired, and cochlear implant listeners. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. I: Stimulus synthesis. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  David A Eddins,et al.  Spectral modulation detection as a function of modulation frequency, carrier bandwidth, and carrier frequency region. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  R. Butler,et al.  Spectral cues utilized in the localization of sound in the median sagittal plane. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Two-dimensional sound localization by human listeners. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  Doris J. Kistler,et al.  Measurement and validation of human HRTFs for use in hearing research , 2005 .

[20]  Anthony J Spahr,et al.  Spectral modulation detection and vowel and consonant identifications in cochlear implant listeners. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  R. Gilkey,et al.  Sound localization in noise: the effect of signal-to-noise ratio. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  J. C. Middlebrooks Virtual localization improved by scaling nonindividualized external-ear transfer functions in frequency. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  C. Watson,et al.  Sources of variation in profile analysis. I. Individual differences and extended training. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  Dorte Hammershøi,et al.  Binaural Technique: Do We Need Individual Recordings? , 1996 .

[25]  F L Wightman,et al.  Localization using nonindividualized head-related transfer functions. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  J. C. Middlebrooks,et al.  Individual differences in external-ear transfer functions reduced by scaling in frequency. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  William M. Hartmann,et al.  On the ability of human listeners to distinguish between front and back , 2010, Hearing Research.

[28]  Douglas Brungart,et al.  Spectral HRTF enhancement for improved vertical-polar auditory localization , 2009, 2009 IEEE Workshop on Applications of Signal Processing to Audio and Acoustics.

[29]  D. M. Green,et al.  Directional dependence of interaural envelope delays. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[30]  John Vanderkooy,et al.  Transfer-Function Measurement with Maximum-Length Sequences , 1989 .

[31]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[32]  E. Macpherson,et al.  Binaural weighting of monaural spectral cues for sound localization. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  F L Wightman,et al.  Resolution of front-back ambiguity in spatial hearing by listener and source movement. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.