Brain potentials reflect access to visual and emotional memories for faces

Familiar faces convey different types of information, unlocking memories related to social-emotional significance. Here, the availability over time of different types of memory was evaluated using the time-course of P3 event related potentials. Two oddball paradigms were employed, both using unfamiliar faces as standards. The infrequent targets were, respectively, artificially-learned faces (devoid of social-emotional content) and faces of acquaintances. Although in both tasks targets were detected accurately, the corresponding time-course and scalp distribution of the P3 responses differed. Artificially-learned and acquaintance faces both elicited a P3b, maximal over centro-parietal sites, and a latency of 500ms. Faces of acquaintances elicited an additional component, an early P3 maximal over frontal sites: with a latency of 350ms. This suggests that visual familiarity can only trigger the overt recognition processes leading to the slower P3b, whereas emotional-social information can also elicit fast and automatic assessments (indexed by the frontal-P3) crucial for successful social interactions.

[1]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  E. Phelps Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.

[4]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[5]  A. Damasio The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness , 1999 .

[6]  T. Schormann,et al.  Functional delineation of the human occipito-temporal areas related to face and scene processing. A PET study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[8]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[9]  M. Crommelinck,et al.  Discrimination of emotional facial expressions in a visual oddball task: an ERP study , 2002, Biological Psychology.

[10]  G. Rainer,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  T. Valentine,et al.  Face recognition and emotional valence: processing without awareness by neurologically intact participants does not simulate covert recognition in prosopagnosia , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  M. Bradley,et al.  Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[13]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Involuntary Attention to Acoustic Novelty and Change , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Blair,et al.  An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[16]  F. Lopera,et al.  Brain potentials reflect residual face processing in a case of prosopagnosia , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[17]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[18]  T. Schormann,et al.  Activation Reduction in Anterior Temporal Cortices during Repeated Recognition of Faces of Personal Acquaintances , 2001, NeuroImage.

[19]  Ken A. Paller,et al.  Frontal Brain Activity during Episodic and Semantic Retrieval: Insights from Event-Related Potentials , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Auditory distraction: event-related potential and behavioral indices , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  R. Bauer,et al.  Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  M. Eimer Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  R. Knight,et al.  Neural origins of the P300. , 2000, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[24]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Multiple sources of P3b associated with different types of information. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[25]  J. Polich Detection of change : event-related potential and fMRI findings , 2003 .

[26]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Neural substrates participating in acquisition of facial familiarity: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[27]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  M. Eimer Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  Bruno Debruille,et al.  Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  A. Young,et al.  Understanding face recognition. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[31]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  C. Nelson,et al.  Event-related potentials in year-old infants: relations with emotionality and cortisol. , 1994, Child development.

[33]  A. Damasio,et al.  Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. , 1985, Science.

[34]  Richard S. Lazarus,et al.  On the Primacy of Cognition. , 1984 .

[35]  J. Haxby,et al.  Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  C. Price,et al.  Identification of famous faces and buildings: a functional neuroimaging study of semantically unique items. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  Neural sensitivity to human voices: ERP evidence of task and attentional influences. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[39]  K. Zilles,et al.  The neural correlates of person familiarity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  L. Pessoa To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[42]  R. Verleger On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[43]  R. Biscay,et al.  Testing topographic differences between event related brain potentials by using non-parametric combinations of permutation tests. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Neural Systems Underlying the Recognition of Familiar and Newly Learned Faces , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  E. Rolls The representation of information about faces in the temporal and frontal lobes , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  The functionally defined right occipital and fusiform “face areas” discriminate novel from visually familiar faces , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  Michael Erb,et al.  Successful episodic memory retrieval of newly learned faces activates a left fronto-parietal network. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[48]  S. Bentin,et al.  Processing specificity for human voice stimuli: electrophysiological evidence , 2001, Neuroreport.

[49]  A. Burton,et al.  Event-related brain potential evidence for a response of inferior temporal cortex to familiar face repetitions. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[50]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  L. Deouell,et al.  STRUCTURAL ENCODING AND IDENTIFICATION IN FACE PROCESSING: ERP EVIDENCE FOR SEPARATE MECHANISMS , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[52]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  E. Aubert,et al.  Associative ERP effects with memories of artificial faces. , 1994, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[54]  R. Henson,et al.  Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition and priming. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.