The Role of Knowledge of Results Frequency in Learning Through Observation

The authors examined whether reduced knowledge of results (KR) frequency during observation of a model's performance enhances learning. As they viewed a timing task, observers (n = 54) received KR about the model's performance on each trial (100% KR) or on 1 out of 3 trials (33% KR). Controls (n = 18) received only physical practice; they did not take part in the observation session. The authors also wanted to dissociate the guidance effect of KR during physical practice from the guidance role played by the representation acquired during observation. Therefore, following the observation phase, participants physically performed the task with either the same or a different KR frequency than that experienced during observation. The effects of observation and physical practice on learning were assessed in delayed retention tests. The beneficial effect of reduced KR frequency during observation continued for the following physical practice phases. Possible explanations as to why KR influences observational learning are discussed.

[1]  L. Proteau,et al.  Cognitive Processes Underlying Observational Learning of Motor Skills , 1999 .

[2]  L Proteau,et al.  On the cognitive processes underlying contextual interference and observational learning. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[3]  R. A. Schmidt,et al.  Feedback-Induced Variability and the Learning of Generalized Motor Programs. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  Diane Ross,et al.  The effect of auditory and visual models on acquisition of a timing task , 1985 .

[5]  L Proteau,et al.  On the Cognitive Basis of Observational Learning: Development of Mechanisms for the Detection and Correction of Errors , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  G. Wulf,et al.  Type of KR and KR frequency effects on motor learning , 1996 .

[7]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Frequent Augmented Feedback Can Degrade Learning: Evidence and Interpretations , 1991 .

[8]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Enhancing Training Efficiency and Effectiveness Through the Use of Dyad Training. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  Y. Blandin L'apprentissage par observation d'habiletés motrices: un processus d'apprentissage spécifique? , 2002 .

[10]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[11]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[13]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[14]  M. Jeannerod TO ACT OR NOT TO ACT : PERSPECTIVES ON THE REPRESENTATION OF ACTIONS , 1999 .

[15]  G Wulf,et al.  Reduced feedback frequency enhances generalized motor program learning but not parameterization learning. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  R. A. Schmidt,et al.  Reducing Knowledge of Results About Relative Versus Absolute Timing: Differential Effects on Learning. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[17]  Heather Carnahan,et al.  Bandwidth Knowledge of Results and Motor Learning: More than Just a Relative Frequency Effect , 1990 .

[18]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Effects of an Auditory Model on the Learning of Relative and Absolute Timing , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  G Wulf,et al.  Optimizing Generalized Motor Program and Parameter Learning , 2000, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[20]  J. A. Adams Use of the model's knowledge of results to increase the observer's performance , 1986 .

[21]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Luc Proteau,et al.  The Role of Scheduling in Learning Through Observation , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  R. Schmidt A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. , 1975 .

[24]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Retrieval practice and the maintenance of knowledge. , 1988 .

[25]  S. Vogt,et al.  On relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[26]  M. Buekers,et al.  The Role of Task Experience and Prior Knowledge for Detecting Invalid Augmented Feedback while Learning a Motor Skill , 1995 .

[27]  C H Shea,et al.  Generalized Motor Program (GMP) Learning: Effects of Reduced Frequency of Knowledge of Results and Practice Variability. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  Charles H. Shea,et al.  Reduced-Frequency Concurrent and Terminal Feedback: A Test of the Guidance Hypothesis , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  G Wulf,et al.  Physical and Observational Practice Afford Unique Learning Opportunities , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[30]  D L Wright,et al.  Cognitive processes related to contextual interference and observational learning: a replication of Blandin, Proteau and Alain (1994). , 1997, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[31]  H Zelaznik,et al.  Feedback in response recognition and production. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .