How Does Space Interact with Numbers
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.
[2] M. Casey,et al. Spatial-Mechanical Reasoning Skills versus Mathematics Self-Confidence as Mediators of Gender Differences on Mathematics Subtests Using Cross-National Gender-Based Items , 2001 .
[3] D. Uttal,et al. The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.
[4] Wim Fias,et al. The impact of verbal working memory on number-space associations. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[5] B. Rourke. Nonverbal Learning Disabilities: The Syndrome and the Model , 1989 .
[6] John Bynner,et al. Numeracy and employment , 1997 .
[7] Pierre Pica,et al. Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures , 2008, Science.
[8] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.
[9] Andrea Facoetti,et al. Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.
[10] Brian Butterworth,et al. Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students , 2004, Cognition.
[11] Laura G. Barron,et al. Relative Validity of Distinct Spatial Abilities: An Example with Implications for Diversity , 2013 .
[12] Clarissa A. Thompson,et al. Early development of spatial-numeric associations: evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental science.
[13] W. Fias,et al. Number sense in children with visuospatial disabilities: Orientation of the mental number line , 2005 .
[14] C. Gallistel,et al. Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] C. Cornoldi,et al. Arithmetic Difficulties in Children With Visuospatial Learning Disability (VLD) , 2003, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.
[16] C. Cornoldi,et al. An analysis of the criteria used to diagnose children with Nonverbal Learning Disability (NLD) , 2014, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.
[17] W. Gevers,et al. The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line , 2008, Cognition.
[18] Ulrike Cress,et al. Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[19] R. Siegler,et al. The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.
[20] P. Nichelli,et al. Right hemisphere developmental learning disability: A case study , 1995 .
[21] Amy Devine,et al. Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment☆ , 2013, Cortex.
[22] K. Wynn,et al. Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.
[23] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[24] Koleen McCrink,et al. Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[25] A. Zivotofsky,et al. Bisections in two languages: When number processing, spatial representation, and habitual reading direction interact , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[26] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[27] W. Fias,et al. Semantic Influences on Feature-Based Attention Due to Overlap of Neural Circuits , 2002, Cortex.
[28] M. Casey,et al. The Influence of Spatial Ability on Gender Differences in Mathematics College Entrance Test Scores across Diverse Samples. , 1995 .
[29] S. Dehaene,et al. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[30] S. Rotzer,et al. Dysfunctional neural network of spatial working memory contributes to developmental dyscalculia , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Delinda van Garderen,et al. Spatial Visualization, Visual Imagery, and Mathematical Problem Solving of Students With Varying Abilities , 2006 .
[32] F. Restle. Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .
[33] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[34] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Précis of The Number Sense , 2001 .
[35] Wim Gevers,et al. Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[36] Freya Acar,et al. A Working Memory Account of the Interaction between Numbers and Spatial Attention , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[37] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[38] Bert De Smedt,et al. Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[39] M. Noël,et al. Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.
[40] J. Lammertyn,et al. The hunt for SNARC , 2005 .
[41] B. Reynvoet,et al. Automatic number priming effects in adults with and without mathematical learning disabilities , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[42] Geetha B. Ramani,et al. Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. , 2008, Child development.
[43] D. Lubinski,et al. Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. , 2009 .
[44] Elida V. Laski,et al. Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. , 2007, Child development.
[45] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[46] Marco Zorzi,et al. Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.
[47] David C. Geary,et al. External validation of the strategy choice model for addition , 1989 .
[48] Hilary C Barth,et al. The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.
[49] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .
[50] Mark H. Ashcraft,et al. Cognitive arithmetic: Evidence for retrieval and decision processes in mental addition. , 1978 .
[51] Mauro Pesenti,et al. Interference of lateralized distractors on arithmetic problem solving: a functional role for attention shifts in mental calculation , 2016, Psychological research.
[52] Tracy Packiam Alloway,et al. Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. , 2007, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[53] Geetha B. Ramani,et al. Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. , 2009 .
[54] Juha Silvanto,et al. The mental number line modulates visual cortical excitability , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.
[55] J. Opfer,et al. How Numbers Bias Preschoolers’ Spatial Search , 2011 .
[56] Julie L. Booth,et al. Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.
[57] S. Shaki,et al. Numbers and space: Associations and dissociations , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[58] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[59] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Reading space into numbers – a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect , 2008, Cognition.
[60] Stéphane Rusinek,et al. L’outil « estimateur », la ligne numérique mentale et les habiletés arithmétiques , 2010 .
[61] S. Dehaene,et al. Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: Symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.
[62] Daniela Lucangeli,et al. Spatial Working Memory and Arithmetic Deficits in Children With Nonverbal Learning Difficulties , 2010, Journal of learning disabilities.
[63] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[64] Kelly S. Mix,et al. The relation between space and math: developmental and educational implications. , 2012, Advances in child development and behavior.
[65] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[66] Justin Halberda,et al. Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.
[67] Giuseppe Vallar,et al. Visualizing numbers in the mind's eye: The role of visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[68] Wim Fias,et al. Shared spatial representations for numbers and space: the reversal of the SNARC and the Simon effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue” , 1992, Cognition.
[70] Julie L. Booth,et al. Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.
[71] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[72] Nancy C. Jordan,et al. Dyscalculia from a developmental and differential perspective , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[73] Wim Fias,et al. Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[74] C. Semenza,et al. The role of working memory in the association between number magnitude and space. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[75] Maciej Haman,et al. The spatial-numerical congruity effect in preschoolers. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[76] S. Dehaene,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[77] Wim Fias,et al. Spatial representation of number , 2014 .
[78] T. Alloway,et al. Working Memory and Learning in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder and Specific Language Impairment , 2008, Journal of learning disabilities.
[79] Julie L. Booth,et al. Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.
[80] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[81] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[82] M. H. Fischer,et al. Number processing induces spatial performance biases , 2001, Neurology.
[83] K. Moeller,et al. A general number-to-space mapping deficit in developmental dyscalculia. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.
[84] Ghislain Deslongchamps,et al. When do spatial abilities support student comprehension of STEM visualizations? , 2013, Cognitive Processing.
[85] K. Priftis,et al. Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.
[86] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Spatial representations in number processing--evidence from a pointing task , 2003 .
[87] Hermann Bulf,et al. Small on the left, large on the right: numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. , 2016, Developmental science.
[88] Marco Zorzi,et al. Explicit versus Implicit Processing of Representational Space in Neglect: Dissociations in Accessing the Mental Number Line , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[89] U. Cress,et al. How to rapidly construct a spatial-numerical representation in preliterate children (at least temporarily). , 2016, Developmental science.
[90] G. Vallar,et al. Tapping effects on numerical bisection , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[91] P. Gordon. Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.
[92] F. Rivera-batiz,et al. Quantitative Literacy and the Likelihood of Employment among Young Adults in the United States , 1992 .
[93] Yves Rossetti,et al. Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[94] S. Dehaene,et al. Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.
[95] B. Rourke,et al. Disabilities of Arithmetic and Mathematical Reasoning , 1997, Journal of learning disabilities.
[96] Virginie Crollen,et al. Spatial and numerical processing in children with high and low visuospatial abilities. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[97] Pieter Reitsma,et al. Developing access to number magnitude: a study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[98] J. Moss,et al. Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: A randomized controlled study , 2015, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.
[99] T. Vecchi,et al. Blind individuals show pseudoneglect in bisecting numerical intervals , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[100] Patrik Vuilleumier,et al. The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.
[101] Guilherme Wood,et al. On the Cognitive Link between Space and Number: A Meta-Analysis of the SNARC Effect , 2008 .
[102] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic activation of internal magnitudes: a study of developmental dyscalculia. , 2005, Neuropsychology.
[103] W. Fias. The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .
[104] M. McCourt,et al. Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[105] Wim Fias,et al. Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. , 2004, Experimental psychology.
[106] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[107] A. Henik,et al. Attention, automaticity, and developmental dyscalculia. , 2009, Neuropsychology.
[108] Michael von Aster,et al. Mental number line training in children with developmental dyscalculia , 2011, NeuroImage.
[109] W. Fias,et al. The development of the SNARC effect: evidence for early verbal coding. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[110] Paul B. Buckley,et al. Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.
[111] Robert S. Siegler,et al. Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.
[112] C. Cornoldi,et al. Intuitive geometry and visuospatial working memory in children showing symptoms of nonverbal learning disabilities , 2013, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.
[113] L. Verschaffel,et al. The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[114] Daniel Ansari,et al. Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[115] Mauro Pesenti,et al. Attentional Bias Induced by Solving Simple and Complex Addition and Subtraction Problems , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[116] C. Gallistel,et al. Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.
[117] Guilherme Wood,et al. Crossed Hands and the Snarc Effect: Afailure to Replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993) , 2006, Cortex.
[118] Wim Fias,et al. How number is associated with space? The role of working memory. , 2011 .
[119] W Fias,et al. Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[120] Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al. Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task , 2009, Cognition.
[121] Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al. A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations , 2011, Cognition.
[122] S. Sorby. Developing spatial cognitive skills among middle school students , 2009, Cognitive Processing.
[123] Wim Fias,et al. Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[124] Sheryl A. Sorby,et al. The Development and Assessment of a Course for Enhancing the 3‐D Spatial Visualization Skills of First Year Engineering Students , 2000 .
[125] Kelly S. Mix,et al. Spatial Training Improves Children's Mathematics Ability , 2014 .
[126] Samar Zebian,et al. Linkages between Number Concepts, Spatial Thinking, and Directionality of Writing: The SNARC Effect and the REVERSE SNARC Effect in English and Arabic Monoliterates, Biliterates, and Illiterate Arabic Speakers , 2005 .
[127] Virginie Crollen,et al. Spatial and numerical processing in children with non-verbal learning disabilities. , 2015, Research in developmental disabilities.
[128] D. Uttal,et al. Spatial Thinking and STEM Education. When, Why, and How? , 2012 .
[129] Sian L. Beilock,et al. The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. , 2012, Developmental psychology.
[130] Lori Andersen. Visual–Spatial Ability: Important in STEM, Ignored in Gifted Education , 2014 .
[131] Marco Zorzi,et al. Representation of numerical and non-numerical order in children , 2012, Cognition.