Motor learning and consolidation: the case of visuomotor rotation.

Adaptation to visuomotor rotation is a particular form of motor learning distinct from force-field adaptation, sequence learning, and skill learning. Nevertheless, study of adaptation to visuomotor rotation has yielded a number of findings and principles that are likely of general importance to procedural learning and memory. First, rotation learning is implicit and appears to proceed through reduction in a visual prediction error generated by a forward model, such implicit adaptation occurs even when it is in conflict with an explicit task goal. Second, rotation learning is subject to different forms of interference: retrograde, anterograde through aftereffects, and contextual blocking of retrieval. Third, opposite rotations can be recalled within a short time interval without interference if implicit contextual cues (effector change) rather than explicit cues (color change) are used. Fourth, rotation learning consolidates both over time and with increased initial training (saturation learning).

[1]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[2]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to rotated visual feedback: a re-examination of motor interference , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Michael A. Stadler,et al.  Handbook of implicit learning , 1998 .

[5]  J. Wallace Nygard,et al.  Cerebral circulation prevailing during sleep and hypnosis , 1939 .

[6]  Peter A. Frensch,et al.  One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. , 1998 .

[7]  Stephan Riek,et al.  The interference effects of non-rotated versus counter-rotated trials in visuomotor adaptation , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Failure to Consolidate the Consolidation Theory of Learning for Sensorimotor Adaptation Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Consolidation in human motor memory , 1996, Nature.

[10]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Laurence R Young,et al.  Models for neurovestibular adaptation. , 2003, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[12]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Adaptation to Visuomotor Transformations: Consolidation, Interference, and Forgetting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation , 2003, Nature.

[14]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[15]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Generalization of Motor Learning Depends on the History of Prior Action , 2006, PLoS biology.

[17]  J. Wixted The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[18]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Current concepts in procedural consolidation , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Kawato,et al.  Internal representations of the motor apparatus: implications from generalization in visuomotor learning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[22]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  Avi Karni,et al.  The predictive value of the leveling off of within session performance for procedural memory consolidation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[24]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Gone but Not Forgotten: The Transient Nature of Retrieval-Induced Forgetting , 2001, Psychological science.

[25]  L. Squire,et al.  Encapsulation of Implicit and Explicit Memory in Sequence Learning , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  H. Cunningham Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Shigeru Kitazawa,et al.  Long-lasting aftereffects of prism adaptation in the monkey , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  J Randall Flanagan,et al.  Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for a single internal model in motor working memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[32]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Spatial representations and internal models of limb dynamics in motor learning , 1999 .

[35]  D. Willingham Becoming aware of motor skill , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.