Disentangling contributions of visual information and interaction history in the formation of graphical conventions

Drawing is a versatile technique for visual communication, ranging from photorealistic renderings to schematic diagrams consisting entirely of symbols. How does a medium spanning such a broad range of appearances reliably convey meaning? A natural possibility is that drawings derive meaning from both their visual properties as well as shared knowledge between people who use them to communicate. Here we evaluate this possibility in a drawing-based reference game in which two participants repeatedly communicated about visual objects. Across a series of controlled experiments, we found that pairs of participants discover increasingly sparse yet effective ways of depicting objects. These gains were specific to those objects that were repeatedly referenced, and went beyond what could be explained by task practice or the visual properties of the drawings alone. We employed modern techniques from computer vision to characterize how the high-level visual features of drawings changed, finding that drawings of the same object became more consistent within a pair of participants and divergent across participants from different interactions. Taken together, these findings suggest that visual communication promotes the emergence of depictions whose meanings are increasingly determined by shared knowledge rather than their visual properties alone.

[1]  Andrew Zisserman,et al.  Very Deep Convolutional Networks for Large-Scale Image Recognition , 2014, ICLR.

[2]  Mike Wu,et al.  Pragmatic Inference and Visual Abstraction Enable Contextual Flexibility During Visual Communication , 2019, Computational Brain & Behavior.

[3]  Simon Garrod,et al.  The Interactive Evolution of Human Communication Systems , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  Jon Oberlander,et al.  Foundations of Representation: Where Might Graphical Symbol Systems Come From? , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  Robert X D Hawkins,et al.  Conducting real-time multiplayer experiments on the web , 2014, Behavior Research Methods.

[6]  Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al.  Common object representations for visual production and recognition , 2017, bioRxiv.

[7]  J. Zilhão,et al.  U-Th dating of carbonate crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art , 2018, Science.

[8]  Gary Lupyan,et al.  Iconicity can ground the creation of vocal symbols , 2015, Royal Society Open Science.

[9]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Referring as a collaborative process , 1986, Cognition.

[10]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Iconicity and the Emergence of Combinatorial Structure in Language , 2016, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Evaluating (and Improving) the Correspondence Between Deep Neural Networks and Human Representations , 2017, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  H. P. Op de Beeck,et al.  Deep Neural Networks as a Computational Model for Human Shape Sensitivity , 2016 .

[13]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Leonidas J. Guibas,et al.  ShapeNet: An Information-Rich 3D Model Repository , 2015, ArXiv.

[15]  Simon Garrod,et al.  Experimental Semiotics: A Review , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  Marcel A. J. van Gerven,et al.  Deep Neural Networks Reveal a Gradient in the Complexity of Neural Representations across the Ventral Stream , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Mike Frank,et al.  Convention-formation in iterated reference games , 2017, CogSci.

[18]  N. Goodman,et al.  Languages of art : an approach to a theory of symbols , 1979 .

[19]  Ha Hong,et al.  Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Silence is liberating: removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. , 1996, Psychological review.

[21]  Fei-Fei Li,et al.  ImageNet: A large-scale hierarchical image database , 2009, 2009 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

[22]  Bruno Galantucci,et al.  An Experimental Study of the Emergence of Human Communication Systems , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[23]  Heinrich René Liesefeld,et al.  Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs(?) , 2018, Behavior Research Methods.