Minimizing the Pain and Probability of Rejection

Some studies have revealed evidence for proximity seeking following interpersonal rejection, whereas other studies have found evidence of relational distancing (Williams, 2007). This study examined whether both processes could occur simultaneously within face-to-face interactions. Participants were accepted or rejected by one person and then interacted with a new partner for the purposes of an impression-formation task. Conversations were recorded and transcribed. Dyads containing a previously rejected compared to accepted participant exhibited higher levels of linguistic style matching and reciprocated conversational content, suggestive of proximity seeking. However, rejected targets also rated their new partners as less kind and reported lower levels of rapport/liking for their partners, consistent with relational distancing. Partner evaluations were statistically mediated by targets’ expectations of rejection. We suggest that automatic proximity seeking and the appraisal-mediated devaluation of new partners reflect efforts to minimize the potential for and pain of future rejection. Recommendations for future research are discussed.

[1]  Stephanie S. Spielmann,et al.  Attachment Theory as a Framework for Understanding Responses to Social Exclusion , 2013 .

[2]  J. Maner,et al.  How and When Exclusion Motivates Social Reconnection , 2013 .

[3]  Ethan Kross,et al.  Social rejection shares somatosensory representations with physical pain , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Lafontaine,et al.  A dyadic approach to the study of romantic attachment, dyadic empathy, and psychological partner aggression , 2011 .

[5]  C. Pickett,et al.  Social rejection and self- versus other-awareness , 2010 .

[6]  Jeffrey T. Hancock,et al.  Language Style Matching as a Predictor of Social Dynamics in Small Groups , 2010, Commun. Res..

[7]  M. Leary,et al.  Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: a multimotive model. , 2009, Psychological review.

[8]  C. DeWall,et al.  Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal perception: selective attention to signs of acceptance. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Gale M. Lucas,et al.  Motivations for prevention or promotion following social exclusion: being rejected versus being ignored. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  W. Gardner,et al.  Benefits of Membership: The Activation and Amplification of Group Identities in Response to Social Rejection , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[11]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  I Am Too Just Like You , 2008, Psychological science.

[12]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[13]  J. G. Holmes,et al.  Balancing connectedness and self-protection goals in close relationships: a levels-of-processing perspective on risk regulation. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  Roz Dixon,et al.  Ostracism , 2007 .

[15]  D. Moskowitz,et al.  Contextual Influences on Interpersonal Complementarity , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[16]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Replenishing connectedness: reminders of social activity reduce aggression after social exclusion. , 2007, The British journal of social psychology.

[17]  Chad R. Mortensen,et al.  Going along versus going alone: when fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  J. Owen,et al.  Self-Report and Linguistic Indicators of Emotional Expression in Narratives as Predictors of Adjustment to Cancer , 2006, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[19]  C. Nathan DeWall,et al.  Alone but feeling no pain: Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Linguistic Markers of Psychological Change Surrounding September 11, 2001 , 2004, Psychological science.

[21]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion , 2003, Science.

[22]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Social exclusion and the deconstructed state: time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Linguistic Style Matching in Social Interaction , 2002 .

[24]  Mario Mikulincer,et al.  Attachment-related psychodynamics , 2002, Attachment & human development.

[25]  K. Craig,et al.  A theoretical framework for understanding self-report and observational measures of pain: a communications model. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  K. Williams,et al.  Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  Marilynn B. Brewer,et al.  Personality and Social Psycholog Y Bulletin Social Exclusion and Selective Memory: How the Need to Belong Influences Memory for Social Events , 2022 .

[28]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Linguistic styles: language use as an individual difference. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  A. Hayes Within-study meta-analysis : Pooling the significance of doubly nonindependent (nonoverlapping) correlations , 1998 .

[30]  R. Baumeister,et al.  The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  P. Shaver,et al.  Attachment, anger, and aggression. , 2011 .

[33]  Phillip R. Shaver,et al.  Human aggression and violence: Causes, manifestations, and consequences. , 2011 .

[34]  J. Pennebaker,et al.  Language style matching in writing: synchrony in essays, correspondence, and poetry. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  Kipling D. Williams,et al.  Chapter 6 Ostracism: A Temporal Need‐Threat Model , 2009 .

[36]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Nonconscious Mimicry as an Automatic Behavioral Response to Social Exclusion , 2008 .

[37]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC2007) , 2007 .

[38]  M. Leary,et al.  Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  Exclusion and Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry. , 2005 .

[40]  Joseph P. Forgas,et al.  The Social Outcast : Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, and Bullying , 2005 .

[41]  Phillip R. Shaver,et al.  The Attachment Behavioral System in Adulthood: Activation, Psychodynamics, and Interpersonal Processes. , 2003 .

[42]  R. Baumeister,et al.  If you can't join them, beat them: effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  Constantine Sedikides,et al.  The Relationship Closeness Induction Task. , 1999 .

[44]  Frank J. Bernieri,et al.  Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. , 1991 .

[45]  M. Main Cross-Cultural Studies of Attachment Organization: Recent Studies, Changing Methodologies, and the Concept of Conditional Strategies , 1990 .

[46]  Linda Tickle-Degnen,et al.  Group rapport and nonverbal behavior. , 1987 .

[47]  R. W. Rogers,et al.  Deindividuation and anger-mediated interracial aggression: Unmasking regressive racism. , 1981 .