Learning word order at birth: A NIRS study

In language, the relative order of words in sentences carries important grammatical functions. However, the developmental origins and the neural correlates of the ability to track word order are to date poorly understood. The current study therefore investigates the origins of infants’ ability to learn about the sequential order of words, using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) with newborn infants. We have conducted two experiments: one in which a word order change was implemented in 4-word sequences recorded with a list intonation (as if each word was a separate item in a list; list prosody condition, Experiment 1) and one in which the same 4-word sequences were recorded with a well-formed utterance-level prosodic contour (utterance prosody condition, Experiment 2). We found that newborns could detect the violation of the word order in the list prosody condition, but not in the utterance prosody condition. These results suggest that while newborns are already sensitive to word order in linguistic sequences, prosody appears to be a stronger cue than word order for the identification of linguistic units at birth.

[1]  M. Tomasello Do young children have adult syntactic competence? , 2000, Cognition.

[2]  R. Brown A First Language , 1973 .

[3]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[4]  A. Friederici Neurophysiological markers of early language acquisition: from syllables to sentences , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  G. Dehaene-Lambertz,et al.  Syllabic discrimination in premature human infants prior to complete formation of cortical layers , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  M. Braine,et al.  On learning the grammatical order of words. , 1963, Psychological review.

[7]  Jürgen M. Meisel,et al.  Word order and case marking in early child language. Evidence from simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: French and German , 1986 .

[8]  J. Mehler,et al.  The neonate brain detects speech structure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Marina Nespor,et al.  On the edge of language acquisition: inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain. , 2016, Developmental science.

[10]  Judit Gervain,et al.  Prosodic Cues to Word Order: What Level of Representation? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[11]  J. Mehler,et al.  Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  R. Gómez,et al.  Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  William P. Fifer,et al.  Two-day-olds prefer their native language , 1993 .

[14]  J. Werker,et al.  Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants , 2013, Nature Communications.

[15]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Carson C. Chow,et al.  Repetition priming and repetition suppression: A case for enhanced efficiency through neural synchronization , 2012, Cognitive neuroscience.

[17]  Stefanie Hoehl,et al.  The use of repetition suppression paradigms in developmental cognitive neuroscience , 2016, Cortex.

[18]  Sharon E. Fox,et al.  Neural Processing of Repetition and Non-Repetition Grammars in 7- and 9-Month-Old Infants , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[19]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[20]  Alejandrina Cristià,et al.  Cerebral lateralization and early speech acquisition: A developmental scenario , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Kenji Interaction-Between Particles and Word Order in the ’ Comprehension and Production of Simple Sentences in Japanese Children , 2003 .

[22]  A. Blasi,et al.  Illuminating the developing brain: The past, present and future of functional near infrared spectroscopy , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  J. Mehler,et al.  Bootstrapping word order in prelexical infants: A Japanese–Italian cross-linguistic study , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  J. Mehler,et al.  Do infants perceive word boundaries? An empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisition. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Hugo Lagercrantz,et al.  Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: a two‐country study , 2013, Acta paediatrica.

[26]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  The development of sentence interpretation in Hungarian , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  Yutaka Sato,et al.  Development of Hemispheric Specialization for Lexical Pitch–Accent in Japanese Infants , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  P. Sachdev,et al.  Animal models of acute drug-induced akathisia – a review , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[29]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Prosody guides the rapid mapping of auditory word forms onto visual objects in 6-mo-old infants , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  The availability of universal grammar to adult and child learners - a study of the acquisition of German word order , 1986 .

[31]  J. Werker,et al.  Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words , 1999, Cognition.

[32]  T. Nazzi,et al.  Prosodic grouping at birth , 2016, Brain and Language.

[33]  G. Dehaene-Lambertz Cerebral Specialization for Speech and Non-Speech Stimuli in Infants , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Thierry Nazzi,et al.  DISCRIMINATION OF PITCH CONTOURS BY NEONATES , 1998 .

[35]  J. Gilmore,et al.  Infant Brain Atlases from Neonates to 1- and 2-Year-Olds , 2011, PloS one.

[36]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  Neural correlates of infant accent discrimination: an fNIRS study. , 2014, Developmental science.

[38]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[39]  D. Stuss,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience. , 1993, Current opinion in neurobiology.

[40]  Brit van Ooyen,et al.  Prosodic structure and syntactic acquisition: the case of the head-direction parameter , 2003 .

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.

[42]  R. Brown,et al.  A First Language , 1973 .

[43]  F. Ramus,et al.  Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. , 2000, Science.

[44]  J. Mehler,et al.  Verbal positional memory in 7-month-olds. , 2015, Child development.

[45]  R. Aslin,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Near-infrared Spectroscopy: a Report from the Mcdonnell Infant Methodology Consortium , 2022 .

[46]  J. Werker,et al.  Six-Month-Old Infants' Preference for Lexical Words , 2001, Psychological science.

[47]  R. Shi,et al.  Functional Morphemes and Early Language Acquisition , 2014 .

[48]  Silvia Benavides-Varela,et al.  Newborn’s brain activity signals the origin of word memories , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  A. Friederici The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  J. Mehler,et al.  Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[51]  Janet F. Werker,et al.  The basis of preference for lexical words in 6-month-old infants , 2003 .

[52]  A. Christophe,et al.  Newborns' Cry Melody Is Shaped by Their Native Language , 2009, Current Biology.

[53]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants remember the order of words in a spoken sentence , 1996 .

[54]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[55]  Barbara Höhle,et al.  Discovering Word Order Regularities: The Role of Prosodic Information for Early Parameter Setting , 2001 .

[56]  N. Akhtar,et al.  Acquiring basic word order: evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure , 1999, Journal of Child Language.

[57]  Fumitaka Homae,et al.  The right hemisphere of sleeping infant perceives sentential prosody , 2006, Neuroscience Research.

[58]  A. Friederici Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  C. Büchel,et al.  Broca's area and the language instinct , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  J. Morgan,et al.  SIGNAL TO SYNTAX : Bootstrapping From Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition , 2008 .

[61]  T. Nazzi,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetries in Repetition Enhancement and Suppression Effects in the Newborn Brain , 2015, PloS one.

[62]  Janet F. Werker,et al.  Language and the Newborn Brain: Does Prenatal Language Experience Shape the Neonate Neural Response to Speech? , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[63]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Frequency and form as determinants of functor sensitivity in English-acquiring infants. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[64]  Y. Konishi,et al.  Cerebral hemodynamics in newborn infants exposed to speech sounds: A whole‐head optical topography study , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[65]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[66]  L. Petitto,et al.  How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children☆ , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  A. Friederici The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. , 2011, Physiological reviews.

[68]  Maria Teresa Guasti,et al.  Language acquisition : the growth of grammar , 2002 .