Executive Dysfunction and the Prefrontal Cortex

ABSTRACT PURPOSE OF REVIEW This article summarizes the cognitive and behavioral functions of the prefrontal cortex with an emphasis on executive cognitive functions and the clinical consequences associated with executive dysfunction. The clinical manifestations of lesions to the lateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal, and frontopolar cortex are reviewed. RECENT FINDINGS Traditional lesion studies have emphasized the role of a brain region in controlling a cognitive function. With advances in neurology, neuropsychology, and neuroimaging, the participation of the prefrontal cortex in large-scale networks has been established with recognition that cognitive dysfunction can arise not only from a lesion within a network but also from degenerative disease targeting these large-scale, dynamic neural networks. Although executive dysfunction can result from frontal lobe injury, this article highlights the role of distributed processes subserving executive functions. An atypical phenotype of Alzheimer disease has been described that selectively targets parietal-temporal-frontal networks important for core executive functions. SUMMARY Executive function comprises working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition and depends on top-down (ie, goal-driven) control of distributed processes occurring throughout the brain. The exact behavioral output (ie, function) depends on the content of the processes being controlled. Prefrontal cortex regions serve key cognitive functions related to social, emotional, and motivational aspects of behavior. The dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex plays a role in working memory, goal-driven attention, task switching, planning, problem-solving, and novelty-seeking. The ventral lateral prefrontal cortex plays a role in inhibition, response selection, and monitoring; the medial prefrontal cortex in self-knowledge, motivation, emotional regulation, and updating goal-directed behavior; the orbitofrontal cortex in personality, inhibition, and emotional and social reasoning. Although dysexecutive syndromes have been traditionally associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex injury, it is now recognized that they can also result from an impaired parietal-temporal-frontal system, which is targeted in a distinct form of atypical Alzheimer disease. This dysexecutive Alzheimer phenotype is characterized by impaired task performance on a wide battery of neuropsychological tests and simple daily tasks that require executive control. In contrast, dysexecutive syndromes more localized to the frontal lobe involve impaired executive control of social, emotional, and motivational aspects of behavior.

[1]  David T. Jones,et al.  New insights into atypical Alzheimer's disease in the era of biomarkers , 2021, The Lancet Neurology.

[2]  David T. Jones,et al.  Progressive dysexecutive syndrome due to Alzheimer’s disease: a description of 55 cases and comparison to other phenotypes , 2020, Brain communications.

[3]  K. Cicerone,et al.  Evidence-Based Cognitive Rehabilitation: Systematic Review of the Literature From 2009 Through 2014. , 2019, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[4]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Model-based lesion mapping of cognitive control using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test , 2019, Nature Communications.

[5]  Frederik Barkhof,et al.  The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer's disease: clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  R. Knight,et al.  Insights into Human Behavior from Lesions to the Prefrontal Cortex , 2014, Neuron.

[7]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease: the IWG-2 criteria , 2014, The Lancet Neurology.

[8]  J. Grafman,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory , 2013, Cortex.

[9]  A. Diamond Executive functions. , 2014, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[10]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of Area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: A lesion study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  L. Fellows,et al.  Double Dissociation of Stimulus-Value and Action-Value Learning in Humans with Orbitofrontal or Anterior Cingulate Cortex Damage , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  John Duncan,et al.  Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  D. Stuss,et al.  Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements , 2007, Nature.

[15]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Does gender play a role in functional asymmetry of ventromedial prefrontal cortex? , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  K. Miller Executive functions. , 2005, Pediatric annals.

[17]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Brent A. Vogt,et al.  Isolated Executive Impairment and Associated Frontal Neuropathology , 2004, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[19]  M. Farah,et al.  Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Jorge Moll,et al.  Morals and the human brain: a working model. , 2003, Neuroreport.

[21]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Factor Analysis of the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe) , 2003, Assessment.

[22]  D. Stuss,et al.  The Trail Making Test: a study in focal lesion patients. , 2001, Psychological assessment.

[23]  I Litvan,et al.  The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside , 2000, Neurology.

[24]  D. Tranel,et al.  Acquired Personality Disturbances Associated With Bilateral Damage to the Ventromedial Prefrontal Region , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[25]  M. Mesulam,et al.  The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  S. Salloway,et al.  Alteration of intention and self-initiated action associated with bilateral anterior cingulotomy. , 1999, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[27]  C. Cotman,et al.  Clinical and pathological evidence for a frontal variant of Alzheimer disease. , 1999, Archives of neurology.

[28]  J. Harlow Passage of an Iron Rod Through the Head , 1999 .

[29]  D. Stuss,et al.  The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[30]  J. Cummings,et al.  Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. , 1993, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[31]  M. Raichle,et al.  Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders , 1997, Nature.

[32]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[33]  A. Kertesz,et al.  Frontal Behavioral Inventory: Diagnostic Criteria for Frontal Lobe Dementi , 1997, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[34]  N. Graff-Radford,et al.  Non-haemorrhagic infarction of the thalamus: behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic correlates , 1996 .

[35]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[37]  J. Bogousslavsky Frontal stroke syndromes. , 1994, European neurology.

[38]  Gabriel Leonard,et al.  Frontal-lobe contribution to recency judgements , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  A. Damasio,et al.  Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  A Baddeley,et al.  The two-component hypothesis of memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[41]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[42]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[44]  A. Damasio,et al.  Nonhaemorrhagic thalamic infarction. Clinical, neuropsychological and electrophysiological findings in four anatomical groups defined by computerized tomography. , 1985, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  A. Damasio,et al.  Nonhemorrhagic infarction of the thalamus , 1984, Neurology.

[46]  Brenda Milner,et al.  Design fluency: The invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  E. Drewe Go - No Go Learning After Frontal Lobe Lesions in Humans , 1975, Cortex.

[48]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[49]  Joseph Adams Passage of an Iron Rod through the Head. , 1849, The Buffalo medical journal and monthly review of medical and surgical science.