Numerosity-length interference: a Stroop experiment.

The existence of a common mechanism for length and numerosity processing was tested with a Stroop task. Participants compared the length or the numerosity of arrays of dots, for which the two variables were manipulated independently to create congruent, incongruent, or neutral pairs. Results showed that the spatial cues strongly interfered with the processing of numerosity, whereas the numerical cues only moderately interfered with the processing of length. These findings reflect a different mandatory processing of numerosity and length.

[1]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.

[2]  F. N. Dempster,et al.  Interference and inhibition in cognition: An historical perspective , 1995 .

[3]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  The Number Space and Neglect , 2004, Cortex.

[4]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[5]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[6]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of how the brain represents and compares multi-digit numbers: spatial and categorical processes , 2003, Neural Networks.

[7]  M. Pesenti,et al.  Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop experiment. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[8]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[9]  O. Houdé Numerical development: From the infant to the child. Wynn's (1992) paradigm in 2- and 3-year olds☆ , 1997 .

[10]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  The Mental Number Line and the Human Angular Gyrus , 2001, NeuroImage.

[11]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[12]  P Dixon,et al.  Numerical comparison processes , 1978, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[16]  W Fias,et al.  Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  J. Baron,et al.  The role of number and length cues in children's quantitative judgments. , 1974, Child development.

[18]  K. Priftis,et al.  Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.

[19]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[20]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[21]  W. Fias The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .

[22]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Imagined transformations of bodies: an fMRI investigation , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Anja Ischebeck,et al.  On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Lauren B. Adamson The neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: Demetriou, Andreas (Ed.). The neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: Toward an integration. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1988. , 1990 .

[26]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Training and Stroop-like interference: evidence for a continuum of automaticity. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  R. Morris,et al.  Sequential judgement effects in magnitude estimation. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.

[28]  Olivier Houdeé,et al.  Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task , 2001 .

[29]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Tests of the automaticity of reading: dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  F N Dyer,et al.  The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the stlldy of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes , 1973, Memory & cognition.

[32]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  J. Dalrymple-Alford,et al.  Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  Jean Piaget,et al.  La genèse du nombre chez l'enfant , 1941 .

[35]  R. Gelman,et al.  The nature and development of early number concepts. , 1972, Advances in child development and behavior.

[36]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .

[37]  R. Shaw,et al.  Precocious Thoughts on Number: The Long and the Short of It. , 1972 .