How well do health professionals interpret diagnostic information? A systematic review

Objective To evaluate whether clinicians differ in how they evaluate and interpret diagnostic test information. Design Systematic review. Data sources MEDLINE, EMBASE and PsycINFO from inception to September 2013; bibliographies of retrieved studies, experts and citation search of key included studies. Eligibility criteria for selecting studies Primary studies that provided information on the accuracy of any diagnostic test (eg, sensitivity, specificity, likelihood ratios) to health professionals and that reported outcomes relating to their understanding of information on or implications of test accuracy. Results We included 24 studies. 6 assessed ability to define accuracy metrics: health professionals were less likely to identify the correct definition of likelihood ratios than of sensitivity and specificity. –25 studies assessed Bayesian reasoning. Most assessed the influence of a positive test result on the probability of disease: they generally found health professionals’ estimation of post-test probability to be poor, with a tendency to overestimation. 3 studies found that approaches based on likelihood ratios resulted in more accurate estimates of post-test probability than approaches based on estimates of sensitivity and specificity alone, while 3 found less accurate estimates. 5 studies found that presenting natural frequencies rather than probabilities improved post-test probability estimation and speed of calculations. Conclusions Commonly used measures of test accuracy are poorly understood by health professionals. Reporting test accuracy using natural frequencies and visual aids may facilitate improved understanding and better estimation of the post-test probability of disease.

[1]  G. Lyman,et al.  Overestimation of test effects in clinical judgment. , 1993, Journal of cancer education : the official journal of the American Association for Cancer Education.

[2]  Lucas M Bachmann,et al.  Communicating accuracy of tests to general practitioners: a controlled study , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[3]  Richard G. Jones,et al.  Indiscriminate investigations have adverse effects , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[4]  T. Perneger,et al.  Does Prevalence Matter to Physicians in Estimating Post-test Probability of Disease? A Randomized Trial , 2011, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[5]  Bruno Dujardin,et al.  Likelihood ratios: A real improvement for clinical decision making? , 1994, European Journal of Epidemiology.

[6]  Milo Puhan,et al.  A Randomized Trial of Ways To Describe Test Accuracy: The Effect on Physicians' Post-Test Probability Estimates , 2005, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[7]  E. Tacconelli Systematic reviews: CRD's guidance for undertaking reviews in health care , 2010 .

[8]  J. Yates,et al.  Seeking and applying diagnostic information in a health care setting. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[9]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  How to Improve Bayesian Reasoning Without Instruction: Frequency Formats , 1995 .

[10]  D. Humphries,et al.  Reckoning with risk: learning to live with uncertainty , 2003, British Journal of Sports Medicine.

[11]  J. Borak,et al.  Errors of intuitive logic among physicians. , 1982, Social science & medicine.

[12]  GERD,et al.  New Directions in Judgment and Decision Making The Psychology of Good Judgment : Frequency Formats and Simple Algorithms , 2006 .

[13]  D. Eddy Judgment under uncertainty: Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: Problems and opportunities , 1982 .

[14]  C. Harries,et al.  Predictors of Diagnostic Accuracy and Safe Management in Difficult Diagnostic Problems in Family Medicine , 2008, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[15]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences , 1998, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[16]  G. Guyatt,et al.  Clinical Prediction Rules , 2004 .

[17]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Communicating Statistical Information , 2000, Science.

[18]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Medicine. Communicating statistical information. , 2000, Science.

[19]  Kenneth F Schulz,et al.  Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios , 2005, The Lancet.

[20]  E. Pujol-Ribera,et al.  Effectiveness of a short-course in improving knowledge and skills on evidence-based practice , 2011, BMC family practice.

[21]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  What are natural frequencies? , 2011, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[22]  R Perera,et al.  Diagnostic strategies used in primary care , 2009, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[23]  R S Hayward,et al.  Users' guides to the medical literature. VIII. How to use clinical practice guidelines. A. Are the recommendations valid? The Evidence-Based Medicine Working Group. , 1995, JAMA.

[24]  A R Feinstein,et al.  Academic calculations versus clinical judgments: practicing physicians' use of quantitative measures of test accuracy. , 1998, The American journal of medicine.

[25]  Ruth Garside,et al.  A qualitative study into the difficulties experienced by healthcare decision makers when reading a Cochrane diagnostic test accuracy review , 2013, Systematic Reviews.

[26]  G. Chatellier,et al.  French academic physicians had a poor knowledge of terms used in clinical epidemiology. , 2006, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[27]  Peter Salmon,et al.  Health professionals' and service users' interpretation of screening test results: experimental study , 2006, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[28]  Z. Bisoffi,et al.  Bayesian clinical reasoning: does intuitive estimation of likelihood ratios on an ordinal scale outperform estimation of sensitivities and specificities? , 2008, Journal of evaluation in clinical practice.

[29]  W. Casscells,et al.  Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. , 1978, The New England journal of medicine.

[30]  K. Matsui,et al.  Quantitative evaluation of the diagnostic thinking process in medical students , 2002, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[31]  J. Steurer,et al.  Simple presentation of test accuracy may lead to inflated disease probabilities , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[32]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Representation facilitates reasoning: what natural frequencies are and what they are not , 2002, Cognition.

[33]  H. Fineberg,et al.  When doctors meet numbers. , 1981, The American journal of medicine.

[34]  J. Knottnerus Interpretation of diagnostic data: an unexplored field in general practice. , 1985, The Journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners.

[35]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight , 2003, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[36]  T. Koepsell,et al.  The influence of types of decision support on physicians’ decision making , 2009, Archives of Disease in Childhood.

[37]  M. Mckee,et al.  Do Clinicians Always Maximize Patient Outcomes? A Conjoint Analysis of Preferences for Carotid Artery Testing , 2008, Journal of health services research & policy.

[38]  P. Vermeersch,et al.  Comparative analysis of different approaches to report diagnostic accuracy. , 2010, Archives of internal medicine.

[39]  G. Lyman,et al.  The effect of changing disease risk on clinical reasoning , 1994, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[40]  Gordon H. Guyatt,et al.  Users' Guides to the Medical Literature: VIII. How to Use Clinical Practice Guidelines A. Are the Recommendations Valid? , 1995 .

[41]  J. Sehouli,et al.  Original Paper: A likelihood ratio approach to meta-analysis of diagnostic studies , 2003 .

[42]  R S Hayward,et al.  Users' Guides to the Medical Literature: VIII. How to Use Clinical Practice Guidelines B. What Are the Recommendations and Will They Help You in Caring for Your Patients? , 1995 .

[43]  Ulrich Hoffrage,et al.  Visual representation of statistical information improves diagnostic inferences in doctors and their patients. , 2013, Social science & medicine.

[44]  Frank Buntinx,et al.  The evidence base of clinical diagnosis , 2008 .

[45]  F. Harrell,et al.  Sensitivity and specificity should be de-emphasized in diagnostic accuracy studies. , 2003, Academic radiology.

[46]  Jane M. Young,et al.  General practitioners' self ratings of skills in evidence based medicine: validation study , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[47]  G. Bergus,et al.  Appraising and applying evidence about a diagnostic test during a performance-based assessment , 2004, BMC medical education.

[48]  C. Schmid,et al.  Why clinicians are natural bayesians , 2005, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[49]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[50]  D M Eddy,et al.  The art of diagnosis: solving the clinicopathological exercise. , 1982, The New England journal of medicine.

[51]  A. L. Cochrane,et al.  Effectiveness and efficiency: random reflections on health services , 1972 .

[52]  Ward Edwards,et al.  Judgment under uncertainty: Conservatism in human information processing , 1982 .

[53]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  How to improve the diagnostic inferences of medical experts , 2004 .

[54]  D. Sackett,et al.  On some clinically useful measures of the accuracy of diagnostic tests , 1998, Evidence Based Medicine.

[55]  J. de Lemos,et al.  Diagnostic reasoning by hospital pharmacists: assessment of attitudes, knowledge, and skills. , 2012, The Canadian journal of hospital pharmacy.