Sleep in the Natural Environment: A Pilot Study

Sleep quality has been directly linked to cognitive function, quality of life, and a variety of serious diseases across many clinical domains. Standard methods for assessing sleep involve overnight studies in hospital settings, which are uncomfortable, expensive, not representative of real sleep, and difficult to conduct on a large scale. Recently, numerous commercial digital devices have been developed that record physiological data, such as movement, heart rate, and respiratory rate, which can act as a proxy for sleep quality in lieu of standard electroencephalogram recording equipment. The sleep-related output metrics from these devices include sleep staging and total sleep duration and are derived via proprietary algorithms that utilize a variety of these physiological recordings. Each device company makes different claims of accuracy and measures different features of sleep quality, and it is still unknown how well these devices correlate with one another and perform in a research setting. In this pilot study of 21 participants, we investigated whether sleep metric outputs from self-reported sleep metrics (SRSMs) and four sensors, specifically Fitbit Surge (a smart watch), Withings Aura (a sensor pad that is placed under a mattress), Hexoskin (a smart shirt), and Oura Ring (a smart ring), were related to known cognitive and psychological metrics, including the n-back test and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). We analyzed correlation between multiple device-related sleep metrics. Furthermore, we investigated relationships between these sleep metrics and cognitive scores across different timepoints and SRSM through univariate linear regressions. We found that correlations for sleep metrics between the devices across the sleep cycle were almost uniformly low, but still significant (p < 0.05). For cognitive scores, we found the Withings latency was statistically significant for afternoon and evening timepoints at p = 0.016 and p = 0.013. We did not find any significant associations between SRSMs and PSQI or cognitive scores. Additionally, Oura Ring’s total sleep duration and efficiency in relation to the PSQI measure was statistically significant at p = 0.004 and p = 0.033, respectively. These findings can hopefully be used to guide future sensor-based sleep research.

[1]  Matt T Bianchi,et al.  Sleep devices: wearables and nearables, informational and interventional, consumer and clinical. , 2017, Metabolism: clinical and experimental.

[2]  Massimiliano de Zambotti,et al.  Evaluation of a consumer fitness-tracking device to assess sleep in adults , 2015, Chronobiology international.

[3]  J. Edinger,et al.  Accuracy of sleep perceptions among insomnia sufferers and normal sleepers. , 2003, Sleep medicine.

[4]  Dawn A. Skelton,et al.  Combined Effects of Time Spent in Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviors and Sleep on Obesity and Cardio-Metabolic Health Markers: A Novel Compositional Data Analysis Approach , 2015, PloS one.

[5]  P. Hanly,et al.  Gender differences in the polysomnographic features of obstructive sleep apnea. , 2000, American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine.

[6]  Junqing Xie,et al.  Evaluating the Validity of Current Mainstream Wearable Devices in Fitness Tracking Under Various Physical Activities: Comparative Study , 2018, JMIR mHealth and uHealth.

[7]  Alan Zelicoff,et al.  Poor sleep quality associated with high risk of hypertension and elevated blood pressure in China: results from a large population-based study , 2016, Hypertension Research.

[8]  R. Roberts,et al.  A Systematic Review Assessing Bidirectionality between Sleep Disturbances, Anxiety, and Depression. , 2013, Sleep.

[9]  S. Redline,et al.  Cross-sectional and Prospective Associations of Actigraphy-Assessed Sleep Regularity With Metabolic Abnormalities: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis , 2019, Diabetes Care.

[10]  K. Avis,et al.  Comparison of a Commercial Accelerometer with Polysomnography and Actigraphy in Children and Adolescents. , 2015, Sleep.

[11]  A. S. Baran,et al.  Full polysomnography in the home. , 1998, Sleep.

[12]  S. Redline,et al.  Racial differences in sleep-disordered breathing in African-Americans and Caucasians. , 1997, American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine.

[13]  F. Hu,et al.  Sleep Duration and Risk of Type 2 Diabetes: A Meta-analysis of Prospective Studies , 2015, Diabetes Care.

[14]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  Relationships between the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), and clinical/polysomnographic measures in a community sample. , 2008, Journal of clinical sleep medicine : JCSM : official publication of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.

[15]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  J. Kiley,et al.  The National Center on Sleep Disorders Research-progress and promise. , 2019, Sleep.

[17]  John P. Sullivan,et al.  A Critical Review of Consumer Wearables, Mobile Applications, and Equipment for Providing Biofeedback, Monitoring Stress, and Sleep in Physically Active Populations , 2018, Front. Physiol..

[18]  Wonwoo Byun,et al.  Comparison of Wearable Trackers’ Ability to Estimate Sleep , 2018, International journal of environmental research and public health.

[19]  H. Montgomery-Downs,et al.  Movement toward a novel activity monitoring device , 2012, Sleep and Breathing.

[20]  I. Colrain,et al.  The Sleep of the Ring: Comparison of the ŌURA Sleep Tracker Against Polysomnography , 2019, Behavioral sleep medicine.

[21]  Timothy J Cunningham,et al.  Prevalence of Healthy Sleep Duration among Adults--United States, 2014. , 2016, MMWR. Morbidity and mortality weekly report.

[22]  Yoshio Nakata,et al.  Accuracy of Wearable Devices for Estimating Total Energy Expenditure: Comparison With Metabolic Chamber and Doubly Labeled Water Method. , 2016, JAMA internal medicine.

[23]  Olivier Mairesse,et al.  Is a one-night stay in the lab really enough to conclude? First-night effect and night-to-night variability in polysomnographic recordings among different clinical population samples , 2012, Psychiatry Research.

[24]  William D S Killgore,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. , 2010, Progress in brain research.

[25]  Marco V Perez,et al.  Large-Scale Assessment of a Smartwatch to Identify Atrial Fibrillation. , 2019, The New England journal of medicine.

[26]  H E ROSVOLD,et al.  A continuous performance test of brain damage. , 1956, Journal of consulting psychology.

[27]  L. Piwek,et al.  The Rise of Consumer Health Wearables: Promises and Barriers , 2016, PLoS medicine.

[28]  A. Goldstone,et al.  A validation study of Fitbit Charge 2™ compared with polysomnography in adults , 2018, Chronobiology international.

[29]  F. Barbé,et al.  Obstructive sleep apnoea and cardiovascular disease. , 2013, The Lancet. Respiratory medicine.

[30]  Riccardo Miotto,et al.  Reflecting health: smart mirrors for personalized medicine , 2018, npj Digital Medicine.