Inhibition of saccades elicits attentional suppression.

Visuospatial attention has been shown to have a central role in planning and generation of saccades but what role, if any, it plays in inhibition of saccades remains unclear. In this study, we used an oculomotor delayed match- or nonmatch-to-sample task in which a cued location has to be encoded and memorized for one of two very different goals-to plan a saccade to it or to avoid making a saccade to it. We measured the spatial allocation of attention during the delay and found that while marking a location as a future saccade target resulted in an attentional benefit at that location, marking it as forbidden to saccades led to an attentional cost. Additionally, saccade trajectories were found to deviate away more from the "don't look" location than from a saccade-irrelevant distractor confirming greater inhibition of an actively forbidden location in oculomotor programming. Our finding that attention is suppressed at locations forbidden to saccades confirms and complements the claim of a selective and obligatory coupling between saccades and attention-saccades at the memorized location could neither be planned nor suppressed independent of a corresponding effect on attentional performance.

[1]  H Pashler,et al.  Do images involuntarily trigger search? A test of Pillsbury’s hypothesis , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  S Zisook,et al.  Saccadic system functioning among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[3]  S. Hutton Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[4]  Peter W Dicke,et al.  Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[7]  K. Fukushima,et al.  Voluntary control of saccadic eye movement in patients with frontal cortical lesions and Parkinsonian patients in comparison with that in Schizophrenics , 1994, Biological Psychiatry.

[8]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  E. Vogel,et al.  Interactions between attention and working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes, it does, but later , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Eye movement trajectories and what they tell us , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  H. Deubel,et al.  Independent Allocation of Attention to Eye and Hand Targets in Coordinated Eye-Hand Movements , 2011, Psychological science.

[14]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[15]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[16]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  R. Klein,et al.  Categories of cognitive inhibition with reference to attention. , 1994 .

[19]  B. Milliken,et al.  Inhibition of return and the attentional set for integrating versus differentiating information. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.

[20]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[22]  J. Pratt,et al.  The Spatial Distribution of Inhibition of Return , 2001, Psychological science.

[23]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[24]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[25]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visual and oculomotor selection: links, causes and implications for spatial attention , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[27]  Michael E. Goldberg,et al.  Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades , 2004, Neuron.

[28]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Selection within visual memory representations activates the oculomotor system , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  M. Carrasco Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.

[30]  C. Curtis Prefrontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[31]  P. Haggard,et al.  Time course of oculomotor inhibition revealed by saccade trajectory modulation. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  A Parametric fMRI Study of Overt and Covert Shifts of Visuospatial Attention , 2001, NeuroImage.

[33]  S. Everling,et al.  The antisaccade: a review of basic research and clinical studies , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Junying Yuan,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2022 .

[35]  J Currie,et al.  Validation of a clinical antisaccadic eye movement test in the assessment of dementia. , 1991, Archives of neurology.

[36]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  S. H. Mostofsky,et al.  Oculomotor abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2001, Neurology.

[38]  R. Andersen,et al.  Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Plans Where to Reach and What to Avoid , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[40]  D P Munoz,et al.  Influence of previous visual stimulus or saccade on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[42]  M M Smyth,et al.  Interference in immediate spatial memory , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[43]  H. Deubel,et al.  Delayed Saccades, but Not Delayed Manual Aiming Movements, Require Visual Attention Shifts , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[44]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  T. Moore,et al.  Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Coherence between fMRI time-series distinguishes two spatial working memory networks , 2005, NeuroImage.