The theory of reinvestment

This review provides an overview of a diverse, temporally distributed, body of literature regarding the effects of conscious attention to movement. An attempt is made to unite the many different views within the literature through Reinvestment Theory (Masters, 1992; Masters, Polman, & Hammond, 1993), which suggests that relatively automated motor processes can be disrupted if they are run using consciously accessed, task-relevant declarative knowledge to control the mechanics of the movements on-line. Reinvestment Theory argues that the propensity for consciousness to control movements on-line is a function of individual personality differences, specific contexts and a broad range of contingent events that can be psychological, physiological, environmental or even mechanical.

[1]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Skill in Sport , 2007 .

[2]  R. Baumeister,et al.  A review of paradoxical performance effects: Choking under pressure in sports and mental tests , 1986 .

[3]  R. Masters,et al.  The role of working memory in motor learning and performance , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  Mark T. Keane,et al.  Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook , 1990 .

[5]  C. Shea,et al.  Enhancing motor learning through external-focus instructions and feedback , 1999 .

[6]  C. Wrisberg The Arousal–Performance Relationship , 1994 .

[7]  M. Eysenck Anxiety, learning, and memory: A reconceptualization , 1979 .

[8]  R. Masters,et al.  Reinvestment and Falls in Community-Dwelling Older Adults , 2008, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[9]  G. Wulf,et al.  An external focus of attention attenuates balance impairment in patients with Parkinson's disease who have a fall history , 2005 .

[10]  A. Beuter,et al.  Analysis of the Arousal/Motor Performance Relationship in Children Using Movement Kinematics , 1985 .

[11]  G. Stelmach Motor Control: Issues and Trends , 1976 .

[12]  C. Shea,et al.  The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  D. BRADLEY,et al.  The Psychophysiology of Sport A Mechanistic Understanding of the Psychology of Superior Performance , 2006 .

[14]  D. Sukhodolsky,et al.  Development and validation of the anger rumination scale , 2001 .

[15]  J. Doust,et al.  Assessment of the lactate and ventilatory thresholds by breathing frequency in runners , 1998 .

[16]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[17]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[18]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[19]  S. Duval,et al.  A theory of objective self awareness , 1972 .

[20]  A. Tattersall,et al.  The Effects of Anxiety on Motor Performance: A Test of the Conscious Processing Hypothesis , 2005 .

[21]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  O. Payton Motor Control: Issues and Trends , 1977 .

[23]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  Paradoxical effects of supportive audiences on performance under pressure: The home field disadvantage in sports championships. , 1984 .

[24]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Online Attentional-Focus Manipulations in a Soccer-Dribbling Task: Implications for the Proceduralization of Motor Skills , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  Deborah Davis,et al.  Use of first person pronouns as a function of increased objective self-awareness and performance feedback , 1975 .

[26]  Lew Hardy,et al.  Implicit and explicit control of motor actions: revisiting some early evidence. , 2007, British journal of psychology.

[27]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  Development of a movement specific Reinvestment Scale , 2005 .

[28]  J. Maxwell Anger rumination: an antecedent of athlete aggression? , 2004 .

[29]  R. Gray Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: expertise differences, choking, and slumps. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[30]  D. Schacter Amnesia observed: remembering and forgetting in a natural environment. , 1983, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[31]  Peter J. Beek,et al.  Toward a theory of implicit learning in the perceptual-motor domain. , 2000 .

[32]  R. Davidson,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory IV , 1976 .

[33]  B. P. Lewis,et al.  Thinking about Choking? Attentional Processes and Paradoxical Performance , 1997, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[34]  L. Hardy,et al.  State anxiety and motor performance: Testing the conscious processing hypothesis , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[35]  R. Singer Preperformance state, routines and automaticity: What does it take to realize expertise in self-paced events? , 2001 .

[36]  Robert N. Singer,et al.  To Be Aware or Not Aware? What to Think about while Learning and Performing a Motor Skill , 1993 .

[37]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication , 1993 .

[38]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Anticipation skill and susceptibility to deceptive movement. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[39]  Nilly Mor,et al.  Self-focused attention and negative affect: a meta-analysis. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  R. Masters,et al.  Ritualized behavior in sport , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[42]  R. Masters Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure , 1992 .

[43]  Raôul R. D. Oudejans,et al.  Anxiety–performance relationships in climbing: a process-oriented approach , 2003 .

[44]  W. Byblow,et al.  The yips in golf: multimodal evidence for two subtypes. , 2006, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[45]  D. Broadbent,et al.  The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. , 1982, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[46]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[47]  B. Steenbergen,et al.  Attentional processes of high-skilled soccer players with congenital hemiparesis: differences related to the side of the hemispheric lesion. , 2008, Motor control.

[48]  Philippe Bolon,et al.  Entropy as a global variable of the learning process , 1994 .

[49]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[50]  H. J. Haricharan To Tell or Not to Tell , 2010, Expecting Wonder.

[51]  L. Hardy,et al.  Stress and Performance in Sport , 1990 .

[52]  R W Cox,et al.  Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes. Evidence from functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  Jean M. Williams Applied Sport Psychology , 2005 .

[54]  B. Hatfield,et al.  Cerebral cortical adaptations associated with visuomotor practice. , 2004, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[55]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  Anxiety-Induced Changes in Movement Behaviour during the Execution of a Complex whole-body task , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[56]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[57]  S. Crowe,et al.  Changes to Body Image and Self-esteem following Stroke in Young Adults , 2000 .

[58]  F. M. Henry,et al.  Increased Response Latency for Complicated Movements and A “Memory Drum” Theory of Neuromotor Reaction , 1960 .

[59]  L. Hardy,et al.  Somatic anxiety, physiological arousal and performance: differential effects upon high anaerobic, low memory demand tasks. , 1995 .

[60]  W. Liberson,et al.  Electroencephalographic Studies , 1941 .

[61]  P. Ackerman,et al.  Motivation and cognitive abilities: an integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition , 1989 .

[62]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  Attention Theory and Mechanisms for Skilled Performance. , 1983 .

[63]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  A Neuropsychological Theory of Motor Skill Learning , 2004 .

[64]  Harry P. Bahrick,et al.  Skilled Performance. Part II. , 1959 .

[65]  M. Maihofer Don't read this. , 2005, The Journal of the Michigan Dental Association.

[66]  Y. Laufer,et al.  Effect of attention focus on acquisition and retention of postural control following ankle sprain. , 2007, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[67]  Explicit rules and direction of attention in table-tennis forehand performance , 2007 .

[68]  R. Masters,et al.  Implicit motor learning, reinvestment and movement disruption : What you don’t know won’t hurt you , 2004 .

[69]  E. Langer,et al.  When practice makes imperfect: debilitating effects of overlearning. , 1979, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[70]  C. Hillman,et al.  Cortico-cortical communication and superior performance in skilled marksmen: an EEG coherence analysis. , 2003 .

[71]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[72]  H. Marsh,et al.  Development and Validation of a Scale to Measure Optimal Experience: The Flow State Scale , 1996 .

[73]  J. Cummings,et al.  The “yips” , 1989, Neurology.

[74]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Performance pressure and paralysis by analysis: research and implications , 2007 .

[75]  C. Y. Wan,et al.  Performance degradation under pressure in music: an examination of attentional processes , 2005 .

[76]  A H FUCHS The progression-regression hypotheses in perceptual-motor skill learning. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[77]  G. Wulf,et al.  Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[78]  Takahiro Higuchi,et al.  Freezing degrees of freedom under stress: kinematic evidence of constrained movement strategies. , 2002, Human movement science.

[79]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  R. Weinberg,et al.  The interrelationships between anxiety, motor performance and electromyography. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.

[81]  T. Clark,et al.  The sport psychiatrist and golf. , 2005, Clinics in sports medicine.

[82]  L. Squire,et al.  Robust habit learning in the absence of awareness and independent of the medial temporal lobe , 2005, Nature.

[83]  Yuri L. Hanin,et al.  Emotions in Sport , 1999 .

[84]  W. Timothy Gallwey,et al.  The Inner Game of Tennis , 1974 .

[85]  Are We Our Own Worst Enemies? , 1982, Nursing standard (Royal College of Nursing (Great Britain) : 1987).

[86]  Peter Graf,et al.  Competition between automatic and controlled processes , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[87]  J. Thompson,et al.  Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos , 2002 .

[88]  J. Maxwell,et al.  Rules for Reinvestment , 2004, Perceptual and motor skills.

[89]  R. Masters,et al.  Reinvestment and Movement Disruption Following Stroke , 2009, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[90]  Sian L. Beilock,et al.  Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[91]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[92]  B. W. Shaw Don't read this , 1997 .

[93]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  The role of effort in influencing the effect of anxiety on performance: testing the conflicting predictions of processing efficiency theory and the conscious processing hypothesis. , 2007, British journal of psychology.

[94]  A. Reber Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. , 1976 .

[95]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: The Allocation of Processing Resources: Higher-Level Control , 1988 .

[96]  C. R. Snyder Coping with stress : effective people and processes , 2001 .

[97]  G. Parfitt,et al.  Somatic anxiety and physiological arousal: Their effects upon a high anaerobic, low memory demand task , 1995 .

[98]  C. Shea,et al.  Attention and Motor Performance: Preferences for and Advantages of an External Focus , 2001, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[99]  P. Sachdev Golfers' cramp: Clinical characteristics and evidence against it being an anxiety disorder , 1992, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[100]  J. Maxwell,et al.  The relationship between initial errorless learning conditions and subsequent performance. , 2005, Human movement science.

[101]  J J Summers,et al.  The role of timing in motor program representation. , 1975, Journal of motor behavior.

[102]  M. Kinsbourne,et al.  Generalized and Lateralized Effects of Concurrent Verbalization on a Unimanual Skill , 1971 .

[103]  A. Salmoni Motor skill learning. , 1989 .

[104]  R. Ingram,et al.  Self-focused attention and coping: Attending to the right things. , 2001 .

[105]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  Motor Performance as a Function of Audience Affability and Metaknowledge , 2003 .

[106]  B. Kendall Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos , 2001 .

[107]  D. Wegner Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.

[108]  J. M. Silva Sport performance phobias. , 1994 .

[109]  A Beuter,et al.  The effect of arousal on joint kinematics and kinetics in children. , 1989, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[110]  G. Wulf,et al.  The learning advantages of an external focus of attention in golf. , 1999, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[111]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The Handbook of Mental Control , 1992 .

[112]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[113]  M. Scheier,et al.  Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. , 1975 .

[114]  Rich S. W. Masters,et al.  ‘Reinvestment’: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure , 1993 .

[115]  A. Deikman De-automatization and the Mystic Experience †. , 1966, Psychiatry.

[116]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis, 4th ed. , 2005 .

[117]  Christopher M. Janelle,et al.  Anxiety and Performance: A Visual Search Examination of the Processing Efficiency Theory , 2003 .

[118]  G. Wulf,et al.  EMG Activity as a Function of the Performer's Focus of Attention , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[119]  B. Abernethy,et al.  THE ROLE OF REINVESTMENT IN WALKING AND FALLING IN COMMUNITY‐DWELLING OLDER ADULTS , 2009, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[120]  B. Chell,et al.  Manipulated Stress and Dispositional Reinvestment in a Wall-Volley Task: An Investigation into Controlled Processing , 2003, Perceptual and motor skills.

[121]  Tsung-Min Hung,et al.  Electroencephalographic Studies of Skilled Psychomotor Performance , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[122]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Mental calibration: Forging a working relationship between mind and action. , 1993 .

[123]  Kelly J. Ashford,et al.  Attentional Focus, Dispositional Reinvestment, and Skilled Motor Performance under Pressure , 2006 .

[124]  S. Keele,et al.  The Structure of Motor Programs , 1976 .

[125]  J. Greenberg,et al.  Persistent high self-focus after failure and low self-focus after success: the depressive self-focusing style. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[126]  M. Bawden,et al.  Towards an understanding of the personal experience of the 'yips' in cricketers , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.

[127]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Neural Basis of Aging: The Penetration of Cognition into Action Control , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[128]  Duration of Parkinson Disease Is Associated With an Increased Propensity for “Reinvestment” , 2007, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[129]  A. Tesser,et al.  Some ruminative thoughts. , 1996 .

[130]  M. Leary,et al.  Hypo-egoic self-regulation: exercising self-control by diminishing the influence of the self. , 2006, Journal of personality.

[131]  S. Vrana,et al.  The Role of Self-Focus, Task Difficulty, Task Self-Relevance, and Evaluation Anxiety in Reaction Time Performance , 2004 .

[132]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Memory and awareness : an information-processing perspective , 1985 .

[133]  P. Ashworth,et al.  Handbook of Implicit Learning , 1998 .

[134]  G. Wulf,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section a Attentional Focus on Suprapostural Tasks Affects Balance Learning , 2022 .

[135]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[136]  M. Corballis From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[137]  L. Hardy,et al.  Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under stress. , 1996, British journal of psychology.

[138]  M. Gazzaniga Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[139]  B. Thornton,et al.  Physical Attractiveness Contrast Effect and the Moderating Influence of Self-Consciousness , 1999 .

[140]  J. Wine Test anxiety and direction of attention. , 1971, Psychological bulletin.

[141]  D. Roger,et al.  The development and evaluation of an emotion control training programme for sex offenders , 1997 .

[142]  Arne Dietrich,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[143]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Depth of processing, context, and face recognition. , 1982 .

[144]  A Ashburn,et al.  A pilot study of attention deficits, balance control and falls in the subacute stage following stroke , 2001, Clinical rehabilitation.

[145]  Joan L. Duda,et al.  Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement , 1998 .

[146]  B. R. Schlenker,et al.  Championship pressures: Choking or triumphing in one's own territory? , 1995 .

[147]  C. Niebauer Handedness and the fringe of consciousness: Strong handers ruminate while mixed handers self-reflect , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[148]  D. Roger,et al.  The construction and preliminary validation of a scale for measuring emotional control. , 1987 .

[149]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[150]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Progression-regression effects in tracking repeated patterns. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[151]  J. Vickers Gaze Control in Putting , 1992, Perception.

[152]  R. Masters,et al.  Self-Focused Attention and Performance Failure under Psychological Stress. , 2002, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[153]  Koedijker J M,et al.  21 Chapter 2 Explicit rules and direction of attention in learning and performing the table tennis forehand , 2010 .

[154]  C. Shea,et al.  Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning , 2003, Psychological research.

[155]  R. Baumeister,et al.  The trouble with friendly faces: skilled performance with a supportive audience. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[156]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[157]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[158]  J. Dimmock,et al.  Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? , 2008 .

[159]  D. Kimura,et al.  Acquisition of a motor skill after left-hemisphere damage. , 1977, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[160]  W. J. Beek,et al.  A Dynamical Systems Approach to Skill Acquisition , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[161]  W. Verwey Effects of extended practice in a one-finger keypressing task. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[162]  Lew Hardy,et al.  Quantifying athlete self-talk , 2005, Journal of sports sciences.

[163]  L. Hardy,et al.  Effect of Task-Relevant Cues and State Anxiety on Motor Performance , 2001, Perceptual and motor skills.

[164]  Irwin G. Sarason,et al.  Test anxiety : theory, research, and applications , 1980 .

[165]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Constraints on grip selection in hemiparetic cerebral palsy: effects of lesional side, end-point accuracy, and context. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[166]  M. Courey,et al.  Risk Factors and Demographics in Patients With Spasmodic Dysphonia , 2002, The Laryngoscope.

[167]  The role of augmented information prior to learning a bimanual visual‐motor coordination task: Do instructions of the movement pattern facilitate learning relative to discovery learning? , 1999 .

[168]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Skill Acquisition : Compilation of Weak-Method Problem Solutions , 2004 .

[169]  G. P. van Galen,et al.  Stress, neuromotor noise, and human performance : A theoretical perspective , 1997 .

[170]  R. Weinberg The effects of success and failure on the patterning of neuromuscular energy. , 1978, Journal of motor behavior.

[171]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .

[172]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[173]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  The evolution of coordination during skill acquisition: The dynamical systems approach , 2004 .

[174]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Attention and human performance , 1973 .

[175]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers' skill , 2003, Journal of sports sciences.

[176]  R. Baumeister A SELF-PRESENTATIONAL VIEW OF SOCIAL PHENOMENA , 1982 .

[177]  R. Baumeister Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[178]  G. Kimble,et al.  The problem of volition. , 1970, Psychological review.

[179]  Justin T. Maxwell,et al.  From novice to no know-how: A longitudinal study of implicit motor learning , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[180]  T. Borkovec Physiological and Cognitive Processes in the Regulation of Anxiety , 1976 .

[181]  A. Ludwig Altered states of consciousness. , 1966, Archives of general psychiatry.

[182]  G. Wulf,et al.  Instructions about physical principles in learning a complex motor skill: to tell or not to tell.... , 1997, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[183]  G. Wulf,et al.  Attentional Focus in Complex Skill Learning , 2000, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[184]  Aidan Moran,et al.  The Psychology of Concentration in Sport Performers: A Cognitive Analysis , 1999 .

[185]  C. Carver,et al.  Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. , 1978 .

[186]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Anxiety and Performance: The Processing Efficiency Theory , 1992 .

[187]  Effects of Anxiety on EEG coherence During Dart Throw , 2005 .

[188]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[189]  Joanne Thatcher,et al.  The Causes of and Psychological Responses to Lost Move Syndrome in National Level Trampolinists , 2006 .

[190]  Bradley D. Hatfield,et al.  Neuro-cognitive activity during a self-paced visuospatial task: comparative EEG profiles in marksmen and novice shooters , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[191]  A. Venneri,et al.  Preservation of golf skills in a case of severe left lobar frontotemporal degeneration , 2001, Neurology.

[192]  R. Masters,et al.  Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing? , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[193]  L. Fasotti,et al.  Slow information processing and the use of compensatory strategies , 1995 .

[194]  D. Gould,et al.  A qualitative analysis of catastrophic performances and the associated thoughts, feelings, and emotions , 2002 .

[195]  D. Collins,et al.  Getting It Back: A Case Study of Skill Recovery in an Elite Athlete , 1999 .

[196]  A. Baddeley Working Memory, Thought, and Action , 2007 .

[197]  Franklin M. Henry,et al.  Influence of Motor and Sensory Sets on Reaction Latency and Speed of Discrete Movements , 1960 .

[198]  Thomas Töllner,et al.  Attentional Focus Effects as a Function of Task Difficulty , 2007, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[199]  D. Crews,et al.  Electroencephalographic measures of attentional patterns prior to the golf putt. , 1993, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.