Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference

Five studies tested the predictions of temporal construal theory and time-discounting theories regarding evaluation of near future and distant future options (outcomes, activities, products). The options had abstract or goal-relevant features (called high-level construal features) as well as more concrete or goal-irrelevant features (called low-level construal features). The studies varied the valence (positive vs. negative) and the type of valence (affective vs. cognitive) of the low-level and high-level construal features. The results show that the weight of high-level construal features, compared with the weight of low-level construal features, is greater in determining distant future preferences than near future preferences. The implications of the results for extant theories of time-dependent changes in preference are discussed.

[1]  D. Gilbert,et al.  Focalism: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  J. Forgas Feeling and thinking : the role of affect in social cognition , 2000 .

[3]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Mixing virtue and vice: combining the immediacy effect and the diversification heuristic , 1999 .

[4]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[5]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[6]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Benoît Dardenne,et al.  Metacognition: Cognitive and Social Dimensions , 1998 .

[8]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[9]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Protecting our minds: The role of lay beliefs. , 1998 .

[10]  Erika D Peterson,et al.  Temporal Adjustments in the Evaluation of Events: The "Rosy View" , 1997, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[11]  G. Loewenstein Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior , 1996 .

[12]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation Failure: An Overview , 1996 .

[13]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Self-control: Beyond commitment. , 1995 .

[14]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[15]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Mental calibration: Forging a working relationship between mind and action. , 1993 .

[16]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Timid choices and bold forecasts: a cognitive perspective on risk taking , 1993 .

[17]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The Handbook of Mental Control , 1992 .

[18]  N. Sanders,et al.  Journal of behavioral decision making: "The need for contextual and technical knowledge in judgmental forecasting", 5 (1992) 39-52 , 1992 .

[19]  J. Elster,et al.  Choice Over Time , 1992 .

[20]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Predicting a changing taste: Do people know what they will like? , 1992 .

[21]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Anomalies in Intertemporal Choice: Evidence and an Interpretation , 1992 .

[22]  E. Higgins,et al.  Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .

[23]  R. Thaler The Winner s Curse , 1991 .

[24]  L. Ross,et al.  Subjective Construal, Social Inference, and Human Misunderstanding , 1991 .

[25]  L. Ross,et al.  The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  L. Ross,et al.  The overconfidence effect in social prediction. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  L. Ross,et al.  Overconfident prediction of future actions and outcomes by self and others. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  Y. Trope,et al.  Activity engagement theory: Implications of multiply identifiable input for intrinsic motivation. , 1990 .

[29]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. , 1989 .

[30]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[31]  K. Fiedler,et al.  The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. , 1988 .

[32]  G. Loewenstein Anticipation and the Valuation of Delayed Consumption , 1987 .

[33]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Discount rates inferred from decisions: an experimental study , 1987 .

[34]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. , 1987 .

[35]  A. Tversky,et al.  Intuitive Prediction: Biases and Corrective Procedures , 1982 .

[36]  S. Sherman On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. , 1980 .

[37]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior , 1980 .

[38]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[39]  W. Mischel,et al.  Prototypes in Person Perception1 , 1979 .

[40]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[41]  A. Kruglanski The endogenous-exogenous partition in attribution theory. , 1975 .

[42]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  W. Mischel Processes in Delay of Gratification , 1974 .

[44]  W. Mischel,et al.  Effects of expected delay time on the subjective value of rewards and punishments. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  K. Lewin,et al.  Field theory in social science , 1951 .

[46]  J. Hunt,et al.  Personality and the behavior disorders , 1944 .