Reproduction of Object Shape is More Accurate without the Continued Availability of Visual Information
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J M Loomis,et al. Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] A Berthoz,et al. Updating an object’s orientation and location during nonvisual navigation: A comparison between two processing modes , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] A. Glenberg,et al. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[4] S S Fukusima,et al. Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[5] J. Philbeck,et al. Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions , 1997 .
[6] J. Philbeck,et al. Visual Perception of Location and Distance , 1996 .
[7] R. D. Easton,et al. Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[8] C C Presson,et al. Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.
[9] M. Jeannerod. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] Ralph Norman Haber,et al. Visual angle as a determinant of perceived interobject distance , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] J. Loomis,et al. Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] L. Jakobson,et al. A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.
[13] J. Rieser,et al. Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.
[14] D. Elliott,et al. Short-term memory for spatial location in goal-directed locomotion , 1990 .
[15] E. Reed. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .
[16] M. Jeannerod,et al. The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.
[17] M A Goodale,et al. The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.
[18] J. Baird,et al. Overflow, first-sight, and vanishing point distances in visual imagery. , 1988, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.
[19] D. Elliott,et al. The influence of walking speed and prior practice on locomotor distance estimation. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.
[20] Digby Elliott,et al. Influence of a No-Vision Delay on Throwing Accuracy , 1986 .
[21] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[22] R. Toye,et al. The effect of viewing position on the perceived layout of space , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[23] A. Patla,et al. Locomotor Estimation of Distance after Visual Scanning by Children and Adults , 1985, Perception.
[24] Michel Laurent,et al. Rôle des modalités de prise d'informations visuelles dans un pointage locomoteur , 1985 .
[25] Bruno Lecoutre,et al. Specific inference in ANOVA: From significance tests to Bayesian procedures , 1983 .
[26] J. Thomson. Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al. Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[28] T. Gärling,et al. Maintenance of orientation during locomotion in unfamiliar environments. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] J. Thomson. How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.
[31] B. Bridgeman,et al. Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] S. Kosslyn. Measuring the visual angle of the mind's eye , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.
[33] M. Potegal,et al. A note on spatial-motor deficits in patients with Huntington's disease: a test of a hypothesis. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[34] Henry Rouanet,et al. Bayesian methods for assessing importance of effects. , 1996 .
[35] James E. Cutting,et al. Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .
[36] R N Haber,et al. Properties of spatial representations: Data from sighted and blind subjects , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] Alain Savoyant,et al. Chapter 2 Visual imagery in locomotor movement without vision , 1991 .
[38] E. Loarer. Représentation et action : l'exemple du contrôle des déplacements locomoteurs , 1990 .
[39] Darren A. Kall,et al. Imagery, memory, and size-distance invariance , 1989, Memory & cognition.
[40] J. Rieser. Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[41] D. Elliott. Continuous visual information may be important after all: a failure to replicate Thomson (1983). , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[43] W. Epstein. Stability and constancy in visual perception : mechanisms and processes , 1977 .