When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation

In the present article a theory is outlined that explains why and when behavioral inhibition alters stimulus evaluations. In addition, some initial evidence is presented that supports the theory. Specifically, results of three experiments show that refraining from responding to stimuli results in devaluation of these stimuli, but only when these stimuli are positive. These findings suggest automatic behavior-regulation, in terms of devaluation of positive stimuli, in situations in which environmental cues triggering approach (because of the positive valence of the stimulus) run counter to situational demands (cues that elicit behavioral inhibition). Relations of the present research to self-perception, cognitive dissonance, and psychological reactance are discussed.

[1]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Beyond Bipolar Conceptualizations and Measures: The Case of Attitudes and Evaluative Space , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[2]  R. Fazio,et al.  The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. , 1999 .

[3]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Associative learning of likes and dislikes: a review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  S. Paradiso The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1998 .

[5]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[7]  B. Gelder Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  E. Harmon-Jones Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[10]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[11]  J. Cooper,et al.  A Self-Standards Model of Cognitive Dissonance , 2001 .

[12]  R. Davidson,et al.  Prefrontal Brain Asymmetry: A Biological Substrate of the Behavioral Approach and Inhibition Systems , 1997 .

[13]  C Andrew,et al.  Motor response suppression and the prepotent tendency to respond: a parametric fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  The Psychology of Evaluation : Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion , 2003 .

[15]  Ruud Custers,et al.  The nonconscious cessation of goal pursuit: when goals and negative affect are coactivated. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  M. Fanselow Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Stopping dead in one’s tracks: Motor inhibition following incidental evaluations , 2006 .

[18]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .

[19]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  Devaluation of distracting stimuli , 2007 .

[20]  P. Lang,et al.  International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings (Tech. Rep. No. A-4) , 1999 .

[21]  A. K. Solarz,et al.  Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  F. Strack,et al.  Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation , 2003 .

[23]  B. Verplanken,et al.  On the nature of attitude-behavior relations: the strong guide, the weak follow , 2002 .

[24]  A. Damasio Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York (Grosset/Putnam) 1994. , 1994 .

[25]  H. Aarts,et al.  Positive affect as implicit motivator: on the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  A. Markman,et al.  The Devaluation Effect : Activating a Need Devalues Unrelated Choice Options , 2002 .

[27]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Unconscious Affective Reactions to Masked Happy Versus Angry Faces Influence Consumption Behavior and Judgments of Value , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[28]  J. Brehm Postdecision changes in the desirability of alternatives. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  F. Strack,et al.  Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Sean A. Spence,et al.  Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[31]  A. Tesser,et al.  The psychology of evaluation. , 1996 .

[32]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[33]  Jane E Raymond,et al.  Emotional devaluation of distracting patterns and faces: a consequence of attentional inhibition during visual search? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Affective Influences of Selective Attention , 2006 .

[35]  J. Brehm A theory of psychological reactance. , 1981 .

[36]  Jane E Raymond,et al.  Selective Attention Determines Emotional Responses to Novel Visual Stimuli , 2003, Psychological science.

[37]  K. Berridge,et al.  Unconscious Emotion , 2004 .

[38]  Hisashi Q. Higuchi On the nature of , 1999 .

[39]  J. Dovidio,et al.  Distance Makes the Heart Grow Fonder : Improving Implicit Racial Attitudes and Interracial Interactions Through Approach Behaviors , 2007 .

[40]  S. S. Brehm,et al.  Physical barriers and psychological reactance: 2-yr-olds' responses to threats to freedom. , 1977 .

[41]  D. Bem Self-Perception Theory , 1972 .

[42]  Klaus Kessler,et al.  Attentional Inhibition Has Social-Emotional Consequences for Unfamiliar Faces , 2005, Psychological science.

[43]  F. Strack,et al.  Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  P. Lang The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[45]  Arthur B. Markman,et al.  The Devaluation Effect: Activating a Need Devalues Unrelated Objects , 2003 .

[46]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  Do Amnesics Exhibit Cognitive Dissonance Reduction? The Role of Explicit Memory and Attention in Attitude Change , 2001, Psychological science.

[48]  Paul Bloom,et al.  The Origins of Cognitive Dissonance , 2007, Psychological science.

[49]  Thomas R. Shultz,et al.  Free Choice and Cognitive Dissonance Revisited: Choosing “Lesser Evils” Versus “Greater Goods” , 1999 .

[50]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[51]  A. Anderson,et al.  Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events , 2001, Nature.

[52]  J. Richard Eiser,et al.  Attitude generalization: Similarity, valence, and extremity , 2007 .

[53]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Neural Systems Underlying the Suppression of Unwanted Memories , 2004, Science.

[54]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Harmon-Jones, Harmon-Jones / COGNITIVE DISSONANCE MODEL Testing the Action-Based Model of Cognitive Dissonance: The Effect of Action Orientation on Postdecisional Attitudes , 2002 .