Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises, predictions and evidence

This paper presents the case for a mapping theory of developmental language impairment, which branches into a theory that Specific Language Impairment (SLI) arises from impaired phonological processing and the consequent disruption of the mapping process through which the words and sentence structure of a language are established. The prelude to the case is that the mapping process, which is a sine qua non of language acquisition, is the first place to look for possible sources of deficits in language acquisition; that recent research on the mapping process points up the contribution of complex phonological processing not just in the segmentation and representation of lexical phonology, but in wider lexical and syntactic development; and that phonological processing is therefore a plausible source of the deficits observed in SLI. Detailed analysis of the mapping process and the role of phonological processing gives rise to specific predictions which are evaluated against wide-ranging research findings on children with SLI. It is argued that the phonological theory provides a better fit with this empirical evidence than theories which posit either specific grammatical deficits or low-level auditory processing deficits, and offers more far-reaching insights than theories which invoke a general limitation in processing capacity. The paper concludes with wider implications, further predictions, and further questions arising from the mapping theory of developmental language impairment and its particular instantiation in the phonological theory of SLI.

[1]  P Tallal,et al.  Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[3]  L. Bloom,et al.  Language development and language disorders , 1979 .

[4]  S. Chiat The role of the word in phonological development , 1979 .

[5]  P. Tallal,et al.  Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. , 1981, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  Eric Wanner,et al.  Language acquisition: the state of the art , 1982 .

[7]  Ann M. Peters,et al.  The Units of Language Acquisition , 1983 .

[8]  D. Slobin Crosslinguistic Evidence for the Language-making Capacity , 1985 .

[9]  S. Chiat,et al.  From conceptual intention to utterance: a study of impaired language output in a child with developmental dysphasia. , 1987, The British journal of disorders of communication.

[10]  D. Crystal Towards a ‘bucket’ theory of language disability: Taking account of interaction between linguistic levels , 1987 .

[11]  S. Pinker Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure , 1989 .

[12]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[13]  M. Rice,et al.  Fast mapping word-learning abilities of language-delayed preschoolers. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[14]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? ☆ , 1990 .

[15]  M. Gopnik,et al.  Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder , 1991, Cognition.

[16]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  The metrical basis for children's subjectless sentences , 1991 .

[17]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  D. Laplane Thought and language. , 1992, Behavioural neurology.

[19]  J. Johnston,et al.  Expressions of past time by normal and language-impaired children , 1993, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[20]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. , 1993, Child development.

[21]  M. Rice,et al.  GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment , 1993 .

[22]  Shula Chiat,et al.  Connections between phonology and semantics: an exploration of lexical processing in a language-impaired child , 1993 .

[23]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The Lexicon in Acquisition , 1996 .

[24]  Mabel L. Rice,et al.  Verb use by language-impaired and normally developing children , 1993 .

[25]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Young Children′s Representation of Prosodic Phonology: Evidence From English-Speakers′ Weak Syllable Productions , 1994 .

[26]  L B Leonard,et al.  Subject pronoun and article omissions in the speech of children with specific language impairment: a phonological interpretation. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[27]  M L Rice,et al.  Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[28]  E. Markman,et al.  When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth , 1994 .

[29]  A. Cutler Segmentation problems, rhythmic solutions * , 1994 .

[30]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Understanding the link between joint attention and language. , 1995 .

[31]  C. Moore,et al.  Joint attention : its origins and role in development , 1995 .

[32]  J R Saffran,et al.  Emerging integration of sequential and suprasegmental information in preverbal speech segmentation. , 1995, Child development.

[33]  M. Tomasello Joint attention as social cognition. , 1995 .

[34]  M L Rice,et al.  Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[35]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[36]  Ray Jackendoff,et al.  The Architecture of the Language Faculty , 1996 .

[37]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Prosody and the word boundary problem , 1996 .

[38]  C Donlan,et al.  Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[39]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[40]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[41]  D. Slobin From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .

[42]  Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al.  Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children , 1997 .

[43]  L. Smith,et al.  Cognition and communication: referential strategies used by preschoolers with specific language impairment. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[44]  Bill Wells,et al.  Developmental word-finding difficulties and phonological processing: The case of the missing handcuffs , 1997, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[45]  Laurence B. Leonard,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 1997 .

[46]  H. V. D. Lely,et al.  Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children , 1997, Cognition.

[47]  E. Clark Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition , 1997, Cognition.

[48]  J. Johnston,et al.  Syntactic bootstrapping in children with specific language impairment. , 1997, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[49]  H. V. D. Lely,et al.  Evidence for a grammar-specific deficit in children , 1998, Current Biology.

[50]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Language learning impairments: integrating basic science, technology, and remediation , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  P. Fletcher,et al.  Resultative VPs and Specific Language Impairment. , 1998 .

[52]  Heather Goad,et al.  Plurals in SLI: Prosodic Deficit or Morphological Deficit? , 1998 .

[53]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Specific language impairment: a deficit in grammar or processing? , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  Heather K. J. van der Lely,et al.  SLI in Children: Movement, Economy, and Deficits in the Computational-Syntactic System , 1998 .

[55]  Ulr Ika,et al.  Mazes in Swedish pre-school children with speci® c language impairment , 1999 .

[56]  J B Oetting,et al.  Children with SLI use argument structure cues to learn verbs. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[57]  P. Tallal,et al.  Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[58]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension , 1999 .

[59]  Shula Chiat,et al.  Putting thoughts into verbs: developmental and acquired impairments , 2000 .

[60]  Shula Chiat Understanding children with language problems , 2000 .

[61]  D. Bishop,et al.  Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: Evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children , 2000, Development and Psychopathology.

[62]  D. Bishop,et al.  Does impaired grammatical comprehension provide evidence for an innate grammar module? , 2002, Applied Psycholinguistics.