Inhibition of steady-state smooth pursuit and catch-up saccades by abrupt visual and auditory onsets.

It is known that visual transients prolong saccadic latency and reduce saccadic frequency. The latter effect was attributed to subcortical structures because it occurred only 60-70 ms after stimulus onset. We examined the effects of large task-irrelevant transients on steady-state pursuit and the generation of catch-up saccades. Two screen-wide stripes of equal contrast (4, 20, or 100%) were briefly flashed at equal eccentricities (3, 6, or 12°) from the pursuit target. About 100 ms after flash onset, we observed that pursuit gain dropped by 6-12% and catch-up saccades were entirely suppressed. The relatively long latency of the inhibition suggests that it results from cortical mechanisms that may act by promoting fixation or the deployment of attention over the visual field. In addition, we show that a loud irrelevant sound is able to generate the same inhibition of saccades as visual transients, whereas it only induces a weak modulation of pursuit gain, indicating a privileged access of acoustic information to the saccadic system. Finally, irrelevant changes in motion direction orthogonal to pursuit had a smaller and later inhibitory effect.

[1]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Effects of attention shifts to stationary objects during steady-state smooth pursuit eye movements , 2008, Vision Research.

[2]  A. Diederich,et al.  Multisensory Interaction in Saccadic Reaction Time: A Time-Window-of-Integration Model , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Sven-Thomas Graupner,et al.  Surprise, surprise: two distinct components in the visually evoked distractor effect. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[4]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Visual-auditory interactions in sensorimotor processing: saccades versus manual responses. , 1994 .

[5]  Uwe J. Ilg,et al.  The role of areas MT and MST in coding of visual motion underlying the execution of smooth pursuit , 2008, Vision Research.

[6]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Neural substrate of antisaccades , 2004, Neurology.

[7]  D. Vaux,et al.  Error bars in experimental biology , 2007, The Journal of Cell Biology.

[8]  Yu Jin,et al.  Preparation and execution of saccades: the problem of limited capacity of computational resources , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  E. Reingold,et al.  Influence of stimulus characteristics on the latency of saccadic inhibition. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[10]  D. A. Suzuki,et al.  Smooth-pursuit eye movement deficits with chemical lesions in the dorsolateral pontine nucleus of the monkey. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Smooth pursuit performance during target blanking does not influence the triggering of predictive saccades. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[12]  R. Gellman,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Kitty Z. Xu,et al.  Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  K. Kopecz,et al.  Saccadic reaction times in gap/overlap paradigms: a model based on integration of intentional and visual information on neural, dynamic fields , 1995, Vision Research.

[15]  Shawn E. Christ,et al.  Motion onset captures attention: A rejoinder to Franconeri and Simons (2005) , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Y. Cohen,et al.  Ocular tracking as a measure of auditory motion perception , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[17]  A. Opstal,et al.  Stimulus intensity modifies saccadic reaction time and visual response latency in the superior colliculus , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Daniela Zambarbieri,et al.  The latency of saccades toward auditory targets in humans. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[19]  Smooth pursuit eye movements and perception share target selection, but only some central resources , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Dynamics of attention during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[21]  Mark W. Greenlee,et al.  Connectivity modulation of early visual processing areas during covert and overt tracking tasks , 2008, NeuroImage.

[22]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for collicular generation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Neural Correlates of Target Choice for Pursuit and Saccades in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2002, Neuron.

[24]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  H. Hughes,et al.  Smooth pursuit of nonvisual motion. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[27]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Laurent Madelain,et al.  Spatial deployment of attention influences both saccadic and pursuit tracking , 2005, Vision Research.

[29]  D. Sparks,et al.  The deep layers of the superior colliculus. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[30]  Jaj Jacques Roufs,et al.  Perception lag as a function of stimulus luminance , 1963 .

[31]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  The spatio-temporal tuning of the mechanisms in the control of saccadic eye movements , 2006, Vision Research.

[32]  M. Missal,et al.  What triggers catch-up saccades during visual tracking? , 2002, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[33]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[34]  M. Wallace,et al.  Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Effects of smooth pursuit eye movement on ocular responses to sudden background motion in humans , 1999, Neuroscience Research.

[36]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Influence of target and distractor contrast on the remote distractor effect , 2008, Vision Research.

[38]  Christopher D. Carello,et al.  Manipulating Intent Evidence for a Causal Role of the Superior Colliculus in Target Selection , 2004, Neuron.

[39]  D. Robinson Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. , 1972, Vision research.

[40]  D. Vaux,et al.  Error bars in experimental biology , 2007, The Journal of cell biology.

[41]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Visual-auditory interactions in sensorimotor processing: saccades versus manual responses. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  S. Shipp The brain circuitry of attention , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  F. Scharnowski,et al.  Long-lasting modulation of feature integration by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[44]  James R Müller,et al.  Microstimulation of the superior colliculus focuses attention without moving the eyes. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry,et al.  Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[46]  P. Knox,et al.  Spatial mapping of the remote distractor effect on smooth pursuit initiation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Monica Gori,et al.  Anti-Glass patterns and real motion perception: same or different mechanisms? , 2008, Journal of vision.

[48]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[49]  L F Dell'Osso,et al.  Audio-ocular response characteristics. , 1978, Sensory processes.

[50]  R. Klein,et al.  A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[52]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  A Buck,et al.  Hierarchical visual processing is dependent on the oculomotor system , 2000, Neuroreport.

[54]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Activity of rostral superior colliculus neurons during passive and active viewing of motion. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Signal timing across the macaque visual system. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Auditory receptive fields in primate superior colliculus shift with changes in eye position , 1984, Nature.

[58]  Chul E. Kim,et al.  Accuracies of saccades to moving targets: During pursuit initiation and maintenance , 1995, Schizophrenia Research.

[59]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Why two “Distractors” are better than one: modeling the effect of non-target auditory and tactile stimuli on visual saccadic reaction time , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  A. Sereno,et al.  Disruption of Reflexive Attention and Eye Movements in an Individual with a Collicular Lesion , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[61]  C. K. Peck,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to visual and auditory targets , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[62]  C. Scudder,et al.  The microscopic anatomy and physiology of the mammalian saccadic system , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[63]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Direct evidence for a position input to the smooth pursuit system. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Voluntary selection of the target for smooth eye movement in the presence of superimposed, full-field stationary and moving stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[65]  Michael P. Stryker,et al.  Eye and head movements evoked by electrical stimulation of monkey superior colliculus , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.

[66]  D. Kerzel,et al.  Visual short-term memory during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  Uwe J. Ilg,et al.  Cancellation of self-induced retinal image motion during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2001, Vision Research.

[68]  U. Ilg,et al.  Asymmetry in visual motion processing. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[69]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition in Voluntary and Reflexive Saccades , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[71]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Spatial allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[72]  D L Sparks,et al.  Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. , 1986, Physiological reviews.

[73]  E. Keller,et al.  Velocity prediction in corrective saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  J. Gottlieb From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition , 2007, Neuron.

[75]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurons with complex visual properties in the superior colliculus of the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.

[77]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Vying for dominance: dynamic interactions control visual fixation and saccadic initiation in the superior colliculus. , 2002, Progress in brain research.